Dr. Oded Rechavi: Genes & the Inheritance of Memories Across Generations | Huberman Lab Podcast

Transcription for the video titled "Dr. Oded Rechavi: Genes & the Inheritance of Memories Across Generations | Huberman Lab Podcast".

1970-01-07T23:11:39.000Z

Note: This transcription is split and grouped by topics and subtopics. You can navigate through the Table of Contents on the left. It's interactive. All paragraphs are timed to the original video. Click on the time (e.g., 01:53) to jump to the specific portion of the video.


Guest Introduction: Dr. Oded Rechavi

Dr. Oded Rechavi (00:00)

Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life. I'm Andrew Huberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. Today my guest is Dr. Oded Rakhavi. Dr. Oded Rakhavi is a professor of neurobiology at Tel Aviv University in Israel. His laboratory studies genetic inheritance. Now everybody is familiar with genetic inheritance as the idea that we inherit genes from our parents and indeed that is true. Many people are also probably now aware of the so-called epigenome that is ways in which our environment and experiences can change our genome and therefore the genes that we inherent or pass on to our children. What is less known however, and what is discussed today, is the evidence that we can actually pass on traits that relate to our experiences. That's right. There is evidence in worms, in flies, in mice, and indeed in human beings that memories can indeed be passed from one generation to the next. And that turns out to be just the tip of the iceberg in terms of how our parents' experiences and our experiences can be passed on from one generation to the next, both in terms of modifying the biological circuits of the brain and body and the psychological consequences of those biological changes. During today's episode Dr. Rakhavi gives us a beautiful description of how genetics work. So even if you don't have a background in biology or science by the end of today's episode, you will understand the core elements of genetics and the genetic passage of traits from one generation to the next. In addition, he makes it clear how certain experiences can indeed modify our genes such that they are passed from our parents to us and even transgenerationally across multi generations. That is, one generation could experience something and their grandchildren would still have genetic modifications that reflect those prior experiences of their grandparents. Dr. Rakhavi takes us on an incredible journey explaining how our genes and different patterns of inheritance shape our experience of life and who we are.


Introduction To Epigenetics And Lamarckian Evolution

Sponsors: ROKA, HVMN, Eight Sleep (02:08)

Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. It is however part of my desired effort to bring zero cost to consumer information about science and science related tools to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. Our first sponsor is ROKA. ROKA makes eyeglasses and sunglasses that are of the absolute highest quality. The company was founded by two all-American swimmers from Stanford and everything about ROKA eyeglasses and sunglasses were designed with performance in mind. I've spent a lifetime working on the biology of the visual system and I can tell you that your visual system has to contend with an enormous number of challenges in order for you to be able to see clearly under any conditions. ROKA understands this and has designed their eyeglasses and sunglasses accordingly. Originally ROKA eyeglasses and sunglasses were designed for sport. That is for things like running and cycling and many of the features they have reflect that. So for instance, they are extremely lightweight. They won't slip off your face if you get sweaty and they can indeed be worn for running and cycling and things of that sort. However, they also come in a number of different aesthetics and styles that make them perfectly suited not just for sport, but also for wearing to the office to work. If you'd like to try ROKA eyeglasses or sunglasses, go to ROKA.com and enter the code "Huberman" to save 20% off your first order. Again, that's ROKA.com and enter the code "Huberman" at checkout. Today's episode is also brought to us by HVMN-Ketone IQ. Ketone IQ is a ketone supplement that increases blood ketones. Most everybody has heard of the so-called ketogenic diet. Most people, including myself, do not follow a ketogenic diet. That does not, however, mean that ketones cannot be valuable because ketones are one of the primary sources of brain fuel and body fuel that allows us to think clearly for long durations of time and to perform well physically. Which is all to say that even if you're somebody who's not on a ketogenic diet, such as myself, increasing your blood ketones can be immensely beneficial for cognitive and physical performance. Indeed, that's how I use ketone IQ. I'll take one or two servings per day typically, sometimes before workout, but most typically before doing a bout of cognitive work. So I'm going to sit down and prepare a podcast or focus on research from my lab or writing project or anything that requires a high degree of concentration for a prolonged period of time. Ingesting ketone IQ prior to that, I've noticed greatly increases my level of concentration and I can sustain that concentration for much longer periods of time than if I don't take ketone IQ. If you'd like to try ketone IQ, you can go to hvm.com/huberman to save 20% off. Again, that's hvm.com/huberman. Today's episode is also brought to us by eight sleep. Eight sleep makes smart mattress covers with cooling heating and sleep tracking capacity. So I've talked about many times before on the podcast. There's a critical relationship between sleep and temperature and indeed between waking up and temperature. That is your body temperature needs to decrease by about one to three degrees in order for you to fall and stay deeply asleep at night. And when you wake up in the morning, in order to do that feeling refreshed, your body temperature actually needs to increase by about one to three degrees. So it's critical that you control the temperature of your sleeping environment and that that temperature be controlled very specifically across the night and into the morning. With eight sleep mattress covers, that's all extremely easy to do. You can program the temperature of your mattress in the beginning, middle and throughout the night and when you wake up. I've been sleeping on an eight sleep mattress cover for over a year now and it has greatly improved the quality of my sleep. And indeed, I know that because eight sleep also includes a terrific sleep tracker built directly into that mattress cover. If you'd like to try eight sleep, you can go to hvm.com/huberman to save $150 off their pod three cover. Eight sleep currently ships in the USA, Canada, UK, select countries in the EU and Australia. Again, that's eight sleep.com/huberman.


DNA, RNA, Protein; Somatic vs. Germ Cells (06:04)

And now for my discussion with Dr. Oded Rakhavi. Oded, thank you so much for being here. Totally my pleasure. Yeah, this podcast has a somewhat unusual origin because I am familiar with your work, but we essentially met on Twitter where you are known for many things. But lately, especially, you have been focusing not just on the discoveries in your laboratory and other laboratories, but also sort of meme type humor that relates to the scientific process. And we'll return to this a little bit later. But first of all, I think it's wonderful that you're so active on social media in this positive stance around science that also includes humor. But today, what I mainly want to talk about is the incredible questions that you probe in your lab, which are highly unusual, incredibly significant for each and all of our lives, and very controversial. And at times, even a little bit dangerous or morbid. So this is going to be a fun one for me and for the audience. Just to start off very basically, you get everyone up to speed because people have different backgrounds. I think most people have a general understanding of what genes are, what RNA is, and so on. But maybe you could explain to people in very basic terms. And I'll just preface all this by saying that I think most people understand that if they have two blue eyed parents, that there's a higher probability that they, their offspring will have blue eyes than brown eyes. Similarly, if two brown eye parents hire probably that they have brown eyes rather than blue eyes and so on. But that most people generally understand and accept that if they spend part of their life, let's say studying architecture, that if they have children, that there's no real genetic reason, we assume, that their children would somehow be better at architecture because they contain the knowledge through the DNA of their parents. They might be exposed to it in the home, so-called nature nurture, that's a nurture in that case, but that they wouldn't inherit knowledge or other traits. And today I'm hoping you can explain to us why eye color but not knowledge is thought to be inherited. And the huge landscape of interesting questions that this opens up including some evidence that contrary to what we might think, certain types of knowledge at the level of cells and systems can be inherited. So that was a very long-winded opening, but to frame things up, what is DNA, what is RNA, and how does inheritance really work? Okay, so DNA is the material, the genetic instructions, that is contained in every one of our cells. We have the set of genes containing the entire set is called the genome, and this is present in every cell of our body, the same set of instructions. And genes are made of DNA, and they also contain chromosomes, they are containing chromosomes, if the DNA and the proteins that condense the DNA, because we have a huge amount of DNA in every cell that you need to condense it to. Sort of like thread on a spool. Right, huge amounts that you have to condense. And we have the same genome, the same DNA in every cell in our body. Can I just interrupt, and I'll do that periodically just to make sure that people are being carried along. I sometimes find that even remarkable, that a skin cell and a brain cell, a neuron for instance, very different functions, but they all contain the full menu of genes and the same menu of genes. No, it is amazing. It is amazing. And perhaps it's good to have an analogy to understand how it works. So this is, I hope this is not a commercial, but this is like the Ikea book that you have in every cell in your body, the instructions to make everything that you need in your house. The chairs, the kitchen, the pictures, but in every room you want something else. So in the kitchen you want things that fit the kitchen in the toilet, you want things that fit the toilet. So you only remove one particular page of instructions, which is the instruction of how to build a chair. And this you place in the living room, okay, and in the toilet, you put in a toilet. So the DNA is the instruction to make the genome, is the instruction to make everything. This is the Ikea book. And in every cell we take just the instructions for make one particular furniture, and this is the RNA. This is the RNA, this is the set. And then at the end you build a chair, the chair is the protein. So the DNA, the RNA, there are instructions to make one particular protein based on the entire set of possibilities. And this is true for one particular type of RNA, which won't be the style of this conversation, which is messenger RNA. This is the RNA that contains the information for making a protein. In fact, this is just a small percent of the RNA in the cell. So we have a very big genome and less than 2% of its encodes for this messenger RNA. However, a lot of the genome is transcribed to make RNA that does other things. Some of these RNAs we understand, and many of them we don't. I think it's a beautiful description and Ikea is not a sponsor of the podcast. So it's a totally fair game to use the Ikea catalog as the analogy for DNA, the specific instructions for specific pieces of furniture as the RNA, and the furniture pieces being the proteins that are essentially made from RNA using messenger RNA. Thank you for that. So despite the fact that the same genes are contained in all the cells of the body, there is a difference between certain cell types. Is it fair to say that there is basically one very important exception, which is somatic cells versus germ cells? And would you mind sharing with us what that distinction is? So yes, every cell type is different because it expresses, it brings into action different genes from the entire collection, and assumes an identity. And so we have cells in the legs, we have cells in the brain, we have cells in the brain, we have cells that produce dopamine, cells that produce serotonin and so on. And we can make different separation, different distinctions, but we can make one very important distinction between the somatic cells and the germ cells. The germ cells are supposed to be the only cells that contribute to the next generation, that's out of which the next generation will be made. So each of us is made just from a combination of a sperm and an egg. These are two types of germ cells. And then they fuse and you make, you get one fertilized egg. And out of this one cell, all the rest of the body will develop. And what happens in the soma, which are all the cells that are not the germ cells, should stay in the soma, should not be able to contribute to the next generation. So this is very important, and it's thought to be one of the main barriers for the inheritance of acquired traits, the inheritance of memory and so on, because for example, like the example that you gave with learning architecture. If I learn about architecture, the information is encoded in my brain, and since my brain cells can't transfer information to the sperm and the egg, because the information is supposed to reside in synaptic connections between different neurons in particular circuits that developed. So what happens in the brain shouldn't be able to transfer to the next generation. Even simpler example, if you go to the gym and you build up muscles, you know that your kids will have to work out on their own. This short out ones won't happen. This is something that we know intuitively, even if we don't have any background in biology. And this is connected to the fact that as we said at the beginning, every cell in the body has its own genome, and the next generation will only form from the combination of the genomes in the sperm and the egg. Even if you somehow acquire the mutation or change in your DNA in one of particular brain cells, it wouldn't matter because this mutation, there's no way to transfer it to the DNA of the germs that will contribute to the next generation.


Lamarckian Evolution, Inheritance of Acquired Traits (14:36)

So despite that, there is, as you will tell us, some evidence for inheritance of experience, let's call it. And here we have to be careful with the language, right? I just want to put a big asterisk and underline and highlight that the language around what we're about to talk about is both confusing and at the same time fairly simple and controversial, right? It's a little bit like in the field of longevity, people sometimes will say anti-aging, some people say longevity. The anti-aging folks feel that longevity is more about longevity. Clinics, they don't like that. Anti-aging is related to some other kind of niche clinics, sometimes FDA-approved or government-approved, sometimes not. And so there's a lot of argument about the naming, but it's all about living longer and living healthier. In this field of acquiring traits or the passage of information to offspring, what is the proper language to refer to what we're about to discuss? There is this idea, and I'll say it so that you don't have to, that dates back to Lamarck and Lamarckian Evolution, very controversial, right? And maybe not even controversial. I think it's very offensive even to certain people. This idea of inheritance of acquired traits, the idea that one could change themselves through some activity, use the example going to the gym. We could also use the example of somebody who becomes an endurance runner, then decides to have children within another endurance runner, and has in mind the idea that because they did all this running, and not just because they were biased towards running in the first place, but because of the distance they actually ran, that their offspring somehow would be fabulous runners. Okay, this Lamarckian concept is, we believe wrong, so how do we talk about inheritance of acquired traits? What's the proper language for us to frame this discussion? We have to be very careful, as you said, and there are many complications and many ambiguities. Maybe you could tell us why Lamarckian Evolution, for those that don't know, is such a stained thing. So it's not polite. Right, perhaps we'll start with just to just say that we can talk about inheritance of acquired traits, the transmission of parental responses, inheritance of memory, all of these things, and we can also talk about epigenetics, and transgenerational epigenetics and intergenerational epigenetics. There are many terms that we need to make clear for the audience. The reason that it's so toxic or controversial is very complicated and it goes a long time back, even way before Lamarck. So even the Greeks talked about inheritance of acquired traits, Lamarck is associated with the term, but it's probably a mistake, although everyone talks about including people who studied. So Lamarck worked, he published his book about a little more than 200 years ago, and he believed in inheritance of acquired traits, absolutely, but just like anyone else in his time. Just everyone believed in it. It seemed obvious to them that it was long before Mandel and the rules of genetic inheritance, and also Mandel was long before understanding the DNA of the heritable material. So this happened a long time ago. Everyone believed in it, including Darwin. Darwin was perhaps more Lamarck than Lamarck. Really? Yes, absolutely. Now we're getting into the meat of it. And this is in the origin of the species. It's in all of his writings. Lamarck didn't even really make the distinction between the generations. He had many other reasons for being wrong, but he connected the terms inherited to acquired traits to evolution, and this is some of the reasons that he was very controversial, even in his time. There were other reasons, for example, he rejected current day chemistry and thought that he can explain everything based on Aristotelian fluids, earth, wind, fire, and water. There are still some people on the internet that think they can discard with chemistry and explain everything that is on earth, wind, fire, and fire. And this wasn't only biology, it was also the weather and everything. So that was part of the reason. But Lamarck made many mistakes, but he did have a full tier of inheritance, which was a big step towards where we are today. So he had important contributions, nevertheless. Although he was a mistake about the mechanism, what he believed, like everyone else, drives evolution is the transmission of the traits that you acquired during your life-holder. The things that you do or don't you do, we talked about use and this use of certain organs that shape our organs and eventually also the organs of the next innovation. He sounds a little bit like the first self-help public figure. Well, this idea, you know, this is heavily embedded into a lot of the health and fitness space on Twitter and Instagram and on the internet, which is that, and it's the idea that we're sold very early in life, at least here in the United States and probably elsewhere, which is that we can become anything that we want to become. And then that will forever change the offspring either because of nature or nurture. Right. And this is a very dangerous idea, as I'll explain in a second, and it led to horrible things. This is part of the reason that this is such a taboo. It's not only self-help, you're helping yourself. The problem is when you apply to others, and this happens in a very, very dramatic and horrible way in the recent past, as I'll tell in a second. So, Lamak, this is what he believed, and he thought this is how evolution progressed, and later, Darwin showed that it's really natural selection, the people of the organisms that are already contained, the particular qualities are selected based on whether they survive or not in particular environments, and therefore, the evolution progresses, they become more common. This is a very different, two different explanations. The most common way this is contrasted is the neck of the giraffes. This is the classic example. According to Lamak, the giraffes had to stretch their necks towards the trees to eat when the trees were high, and because of that, they transmitted these traits. Long necks to their children who also had long necks. By the way, you only mentioned this example, you know, a handful of times. It didn't really focus on that. And according to Darwin, just that a giraffe happened to be born with the long neck survived because it ate, so it's genetic, heritable materials, you know about genetics, but take over, and the rest of the giraffes that have different heritable materials just die. So, this is natural selection versus inheritance for quite traits. There are many reasons why Lamakism and inheritance for quite traits became such a bad term. One of the biggest is what happened in the Soviet Union, under Stalin, there was a scientist called Lissenko who thought that Mendelism, normal genetics, is bourgeois science, shouldn't be done, and whether the normal genetics was either killed or sent to Siberia. And he thought that, just like you said, we can not only we can become everything that we want, but we can grow everything that we want in every field, we can take frozen fields and grow potatoes there and so on. And this led to massive starvation, ruin agriculture in the Soviet Union, also ruin science for many, many years, and put a very dark cloud on the entire field. And only probably in the 80s or something like this, the fields started to recuperate for that. Aside from that, which is a very dramatic thing, there was also crazy stories around an attempt to prove the inheritance of quite traits, despite the realization of many scientists, this is something that is very rare, or that normally doesn't happen, that is not a normal way that inheritance works.


Paul Kammerer & Toad Morphology (22:54)

And I can tell you about two such dramatic cases that will illustrate it. Yeah, please. So in the beginning of the 20th century, in Vienna, there was a researcher called Paul Cameron, who was a very famous and also very colorful figure, who did experiments on many different types of animals. He did experiments on toads that are called the midwife's toad because the male carries the eggs. And there's a beautiful book about it from Kestler telling the story of what happened there. And there are a couple of types of toads. Some of them live under water and some of them live on land. And these toads are different in the shape and in the behavior. So, of course, the capacity to live under water is one thing, but also the morphology and appearance changes. The toads that live under water develop these nupital pads, these black pads on their hands, that allow the males to grab on to the female without flipping. Formating. Formating. And the ones on land don't have them. He claimed that he can take the toads and train them to live under water, changing the temperature and all kinds of things. It's a very difficult animal to work with. Eventually, according to Cameron, they will acquire the capacity to live under water and also change the physiology and develop these nupital pads on their heads. With this discovery, he traveled the world, became very famous. This was just the beginning of the previous century. As the person who found the proof for inheritance for co-ratrates, despite the controversy and so on, and the beginning of the realization of how it actually works with DNA and so on. Not with DNA, but with natural selection. DNA came later. People didn't believe him. He was actually under a lot of attacks, but it seemed convincing. At the end, what happens is that they found that he injected ink to the toads to make them become black, to have this nupital pad. So he faked the results. And he couldn't stand up with the accusations and kill himself. Wow. In this book, by Kessel, it was just maybe it was the assistant who did it. Who killed him? No, no. Who injected it to sort of save him from it because the samples lost the coloring or something like that. So it might be, who knows what happened. Well, in science, whenever there's a fraud accusation or controversy, it's not uncommon to see a passing of responsibility. Right. There are recent cases, there are ongoing cases now where it's a question of who did what, etc. Actually, I have two questions before the second story. I'm struck by the idea that he was traveling and talking. I'm guessing this was before PowerPoint and keynote, but also before transparencies, which actually were still in place when I was a graduate student. For those of you who don't know, transparencies are basically transparent pieces of plastic paper that you put onto a projector and then you can write on them and do demonstrations, but can show photographs and things like that. So how was he giving these talks and would he travel with the toes? So he traveled with the samples. I see. And I'm basing this on this Kessler book, which is on its own very controversial. It's more of a beautiful story than perhaps the truth. But and according to the story there, he had to stand one side of the lecture hall with his hands behind the back while others would examine the samples and pass them around and so on. But he cheated, someone cheated. He probably did it. At least that's what most people think. This wasn't replicated. I mean, also, I don't think anyone tried to replicate it. Interesting. This is just a point about replication and actually another tragic example, not but a few years ago Sakai, who was a, as far as we knew, was doing very accomplished work on the growth of retinas, literally growing eyes in a dish. And everyone believes that result. But then there were some accusations about another result that turned out to be fraudulent and Sakai killed himself. This was a recent, this was only about five, ten years ago. So it still happens. Yeah, it happened. I think it's rare, but it does happen, especially in this high profile situation. I would argue, I'd love to know what your number is, but I would argue that 99% of scientists are seeking truth and are well meaning honest people. I totally agree. And I think that even when people are wrong, it's mostly not because they're evil and trying to act. Like maybe they really want to believe their results, or they're all kinds of way to be wrong. And even to bend truth without just blatant fraud. But this is, according to the story, an example of very bad fraud, which is, I agree, is rare because most scientists, as you said, this is also my opinion. I'm just trying to discover truth and do the best they can. Why else would you go into it? Because it's certainly not a profession to go into if you want to get rich. It's not the money. And it's probably not even a profession to go into if you want to get famous. If you want to be famous, you should go to Hollywood or become a serial killer because they'll make specials. Please don't. But please don't do either. Hollywood, I suppose, for some is fine. But in any case, okay, so camera around 1907, 1906. This is slightly before the whole thing that controversy broke out after the First World War. Okay. Yeah. Great. So camera is gone. His toads with their ink or whatever, nupital pads. They have to go back to mating on land. Yeah.


AG1 (Athletic Greens) (28:52)

Okay. I'd like to take a quick break and acknowledge one of our sponsors, Athletic Greens. Athletic Greens, now called AG1, is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink that covers all of your foundational nutritional needs. I've been taking Athletic Greens since 2012, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast. The reason I started taking Athletic Greens and the reason I still take Athletic Greens once or usually twice a day is that it gets to be the probiotics that I need for gut health. Our gut is very important. It's populated by gut microbiota that communicate with the brain, the immune system, and basically all the biological systems of our body to strongly impact our immediate and long-term health. And those probiotics in Athletic Greens are optimal and vital for microbiota health. In addition, Athletic Greens contains a number of adaptogens, vitamins and minerals that make sure that all of my foundational nutritional needs are met. And it tastes great. If you'd like to try Athletic Greens, you can go to athleticgreens.com/huberman, and they'll give you five free travel packs that make it really easy to mix up Athletic Greens while you're on the road and the car on the plane, etc.


James McConnell & Memory Transfer (30:06)

And they'll give you a year supply of vitamin D3K2. Again, that's at LetItGreens.com/huberman to get the five free travel packs and the year supply of vitamin D3K2. So this is, forget about that. We also had the Lisenko episode. That's a very big thing. And then in the US, there was in the 70s and 80s, a researcher named McConnell who did very different experiments. And he was also a character. He worked on... So he was a joker type of thing, and he published many of his results in a journal that he published that was called "Warm's Breeders Gazette" and had many cartoons. So he started his own journal? Yes. That's one way to publish a lot. But he also published in very respected journals in parallel. But he was a psychologist, American psychologist, and he worked on a worm, which is a flat worm, which is called a flat worm. Which is called "Planeria." It's very interesting. It's different than what we'll discuss today, different type of worm. You know, worms are very common. So four out of five animals on this planet is a worm. Really? Yes. No matter if you just count the individual. So we are the exception. But I'll talk about it very different worm later. This is a flat worm that is called "Planeria." And it is remarkable in many ways. It was also a model that many people worked on, including the fathers of genetics, people who started genetics like Morgan. They worked on it in the beginning. But it's very, very hard to study genetics in this worm. Because unlike us, unlike what we explained before about how we all developed forms of sperm and an egg, these worms, most of the time, reproduce just by fission. They tear themselves apart. So they have a head and a tail, and the part of the head would just tear itself apart on the tail. The head will grow in your tail, the tail will grow in your head. You can even cut them to 200 pieces. Each piece will grow into a new worm. And they have centralized brains with lobes and everything, and even degenerate eyes. He started these worms, and he said that he can teach them certain things, associations, by pairing all... I don't remember exactly what he did. I think it was either lights or electricity with... Shock them, yeah. Shock them with other things, and he could train them to learn and remember particular things. Like, you think you might get shocked on one side of the tank and then avoid that side of the tank? Yes. And then I guess the question is whether or not they're ripped apart cells and their subsequent generations will know to avoid that side of the tank without having ever been exposed to the shock. Right. So without ever being exposed to shock or whether the new generation, the new head, will be able to learn faster. That's another subtlety that you might have. And this is what he said he can teach them certain things, remove the cut of their heads and new heads with all the brain will grow and that it will contain the memory. This was the start of the controversy, not the end of it, only the beginning. Then he said something even much wilder, which is he can train them to learn certain things, and then just chop them up, put them in a blender, and feed them to other worms, because they are kind of a problem. Because they are cannibalistic, they eat each other. And that the memory will transfer through feeling. This sounds such a dramatic feel. And by the way, this opened the field. So people did experiments that not only in planaria, but in goldfish and certain rodents. And did this memory brain transfer essays in planting brain. And this is in the back when they had an idea that some memories could be molecular, could have a molecular form, which is very appealing. It's almost like science fiction. You can have a memory in a tube. And like the way we think about memory normally, which is something that is distributed in neuronal circuits and encoded in the strength of particular sign-ups and so on. But the idea that you can take a memory and reduce it into a molecule and transfer it around is very, very interesting. So this is why it attracted so many people. This ended up in a catastrophe. So there was an NIH investigation. No one can replicate anything. It was a big mess. Although there were always scientists who said, "Yes, we can replicate this in this." So they were in the back. McConnell stuff was different. Again, people thought that they couldn't, that their problems, replicating, but it wasn't necessarily, but some people replicate, but it wasn't necessarily about replicating the whole thing. But the question was, did the memory transfer is specific, or is it an overall sensitization that transmits and so on? Right. Like you could imagine that what gets transmitted is a hypersensitivity to electricity, as opposed to the specific location that the electricity was introduced. Or even more than that, even just in our hypersensitivity in general, you're more vigilant and you'll learn anything faster. That's also a possibility. But his problem wasn't the accusation. It was much worse that he was targeted by the Unabomel. This tell is to send letters with bombs to many scientists for 15 years. And his assistant, again, his assistant, I think, exploded and this is how his line of research ended. Just recently, a few years ago, a researcher from Boston, Mike Levin, and his postdoc, Tal Shomrat, replicated some of McConnell's experiments with the cutting of the head, but using very fancy equipment and automated tracking. They could say that they couldn't replicate some of his experiments. Really? Yes. And they don't open packages in that laboratory. They have interesting stories. You should have Mike over. Yeah, I'm familiar with a bit of his work. I didn't realize they had done that experiment. Yeah, so they published it a few years ago. And this is very interesting. But of course, they don't know how it happens. The mechanism is unclear. McConnell went a step further than this. And what's fascinating is that these are experiments that were done in the 70s and 80s. He said that he cannot only transfer the memories through chopped animals, but he can take the animals that learned and break it down into different fractions. So just the DNA, just the RNA, just the fat, the proteins, the sugars. And he said that the fraction that transmit the memory is the RNA. And this is very, very interesting because it was a long time before everything that we know about RNA today. I'll soon go into my research, explain what we do. And then you see that you can actually feed worms with RNA and have many things happen. This is everyone. No, this is true. So this is why it was so appealing to go back to that and study it. By the way, at the time it became popular knowledge. Everyone knew these experiments. There was a Star Trek episode about it from 84. There are comics books about it, books about it. And people were eating RNA because they thought that there was RNA in memory. This was, of course, complete nonsense. But this was, it made a lot of noise in these years, which is part of the reason it was so toxic until recently. You couldn't touch it because it was considered pseudoscience, likely Senko, like Kamala, none of this. So this was just something you didn't want to touch at all.


Weismann Barrier; Epigenetics (37:31)

And then we go back to these studies about inheritance of memory or inheritance of acquired traits in other organisms, in members, in humans. And aside from the dark cloud that these episodes left, there were also theoretical problems of why this can't happen. Barriers that have to be breached for these two happen. And you can talk about many different types of barriers. And you can also narrow it down to two main barriers. First barrier, we mentioned it, this is the separation of the soma from the German. Right, the somatic cells, they can change in response to experience the sperm and the egg, the so-called germ cells, cannot. That's the idea. Or they are isolated on what happens in the soma. The man who first thought about this barrier is called Weizmann, August Weizmann, this was in the 19th century. So it is called today the Weizmann barrier. Separation of the soma from the German, only the German line transmitting to the next generation. And this is also called the second law of biology. So this is very, very fundamental. So natural selection is the first one, this is the second one, because it's so important to how we work, to how our bodies work. Weizmann, by the way, thought that if you will have direct influence of the environment on the germ cells, then perhaps this could transfer to the next generation. So it wasn't as strict as his barrier suggests, but this is not how most people remember it. But he thought that this is unnecessary. It's possible that natural selection can expand everything. And he compared to a boat, which is in the ocean, it is sailing, and it has a sail open. So you don't have to assume that it has an engine. The wind is blowing, you don't have to assume other things. The natural selection might be enough. So this barrier is still standing, but not entirely. It is bridging some organisms. We'll go into that in a second. The other barrier is the, now we have to understand the other barrier, we have to talk about epigenetics, we have to define epigenetics and what it is. And epigenetics is another term which people misuse horribly and say about everything that is epigenetics, even people from the field. The word itself, the term was defined in the 40s by Wellington, Conrad Wellington. And he talked about the interaction between genes and their products that in the end bring about the phenotype of the consequences and how genes influence development. Later people discovered mechanisms that change the action of genes, different mechanisms, and started talking about these epigenetics. For example, the DNA is built out of four basic elements, the AT, G, and C. And they can be chemically modified. So in addition to just the information that you have in the sequence of the DNA, you also have this information in the modification of the basis. The most common modification that has been studied in modern others is modification of the letter C of cytosine, methylation, the addition of a methyl group to this C. And this can be replicated so after the DNA, the cell is divided and replicate the genetic material. In certain cases also, these chemical modifications can be added on and replicated and be preserved. For those who aren't as familiar with thinking about genes and gene structure and epigenetics, could we think of these? You mentioned the four nucleotide bases, CGA, D. But could we imagine that through things like methylation, it's sort of like taking the primary colors and adding a, changing one of them a little bit, changing the hue just slightly, which then opens up an enormous number of new options of colored integration. Absolutely. It's just more combinations, more ways, more information. There are the modifications of the DNA, and also there are the modifications of the proteins which condense the DNA that are called histones. So they are also modified by many different chemicals. Again, methylation is a very common modification of the acetylation, even serotonin, the serotonin of histones. Serotonin. This is a new paper from Nature from a few years ago. Can change DNA? Not the DNA itself, but the protein that condenses it. Essentially how in the analogy I used before of how the thread is wrapped around the spool essentially. Yes. And this determines the degree of condensation of the DNA, whether the gene is now more or less accessible and therefore can perhaps be expressed more or less. This is one way to affect the gene expression and bring about the function of the gene. There are many additional ways, not the only one. So then when all of these were starting to be lucid, people talked about epigenetics. I started talking about these modifications. I forgot the original definition. And when people said epigenetics, they talk about methylation and things like that. And again, to just frame this up so we can imagine two identical twins, so called monozygotic twins. We could go a step further and say that they're monochorionic and they were in the same placental sac because twins can be raised in separate sacs, slightly different early environments. Let's say those two twins are raised separately. One experiences certain things. The other things, they eat different foods, etc. And there is the possibility through epigenetic mechanisms that through methylation and acetylation, serotonin, production, etc. That the expression of certain genes in one of the twins could be amplified relative to the other. So we know that even I totally identical twins, genetically, they're identical, but they look different and they are different. We experience, we all experience it. And this can happen because of these epigenetic changes. Or it can happen because of other mechanisms. Because genes respond to the environment, genes don't exist in a vacuum. So genes need to be activated by transcription factors. There's a lot of machinery that is responsible for making genes function. So we are a combination of our genetic material and the environment. So when people talk about epigenetics and talk just about the modification, they're also not exactly right. My definition of epigenetics is inheritance, which occurs either across cell division or more interestingly also for this podcast now, across generations. Not because of changes to the DNA sequence, but through other mechanisms. I think this is the most robust definition that allows you to understand what you're talking about. And then again, and then the question is, if this happens, then what are the molecules that actually transmit information and carcinogens? Are they these chemical modifications to the DNA or to the proteins that condense the DNA?


Epigenetic Reprogramming; Imprinted Genes (45:13)

Or are there other agents that transmit information? And which molecules can do it? And I actually think that the most interesting players today are RNA molecules. But before I go into that, I just want to say that when we talk about the barriers to epigenetic inheritance or the barriers to inheritance of acquired traits, in addition to the separation of the summer from the German that we discussed, the other main barrier, it's called epigenetic reprogramming. And the other thing which is that we acquired our genetic material in our cells, acquires all kinds of changes, these chemical changes, modifications we discussed. But these modifications are largely erased in the transition between generations. So in the German, in the spearmand egg, and also in the early embryo, most of them modifications are removed, so we can start a blank slate. Based on the genetic instructions. And this is crucial. Otherwise, according to the theory, it's not clear that actually true because in some organisms, it doesn't really happen. We will just, we will not develop according to the species typical genetic instructions. So to preserve this, we raise the modifications that start anew. And this is in members and in humans. This is allowed to, to most of the modifications on the sperm and in the egg are removed, so about 90% of them. Some remain, which could be interesting. So the idea, if I understand correctly, is that there's some advantage to wiping the slate clean and returning to the original plant. In, in the context of the IKEA furniture analogy, the instruction book is the one that's issued to everybody, okay, or every cell, right? Only certain instructions are used for certain cells, say a skin cell or a neuron or a liver cell or any other cell for that matter. Through the course of the lifespan of the organism, those specific instructions are adjusted somewhat. Okay, so maybe like IKEA furniture, sometimes you, you can't, they send you seven, not eight of particular cells. Or they send you the proper number, but you put them in the wrong place and it sort of changes the way that the thing works a little bit. Once that assuming furniture could reproduce, but here in the analogy of the furniture as, as the cell or the organ in that makes with another organism, that needs to be replicated. And so the idea is to take the instruction, but go through and erase all the pen and pencil marks, erase all those additional little modifications that the owner used or introduced to it and return to the original instruction. Right, because if you want to bring back the instruction book, you want it to have all the potential to make all the furniture. You normally to be restricted to the ones that you made in the particular room. So it's essentially the opposite of acquired traits and characteristics based on your, what we say in biology, geek speak, lineage based experience, but what your parents experience, right? In some ways we want to eliminate all that and go back to just the genes they provided. Yes, but it's more complicated. It's more complicated than that because we have some very striking examples, even in members, where some of the marks are maintained, for example, the classic example is imprinted. Imprinting is a very interesting phenomena. The way DNA works is that you inherit a copy for every chromosome from your mother and your father. And then you have in every cell of your body two copies, if you're a human, of every chromosome. And then, so every gene is represented twice. These are called alleles, the different versions of the genes. And the thought is that once you enter in the next generation, the two copies that you inherited are equal. It doesn't matter whether you acquire them from your mother or from your father, right? There are some situations where it does matter. There is a limited number of genes that are called imprinting, where it does matter whether you inherited from your mother or your father. And this is happening through epigenetic inheritance, not because of changes to the DNA sequence, but because of maintenance of these chemical modifications across generations. And as I recall from the beautiful work of Catherine Doolock at Harvard, that especially in the brain, there is evidence that some cells contain the complete genome from mom or the complete genome from dad. And it can also switch during your life. So for her work show that early on in your life, it's different whether you express the maternal or paternal copy than when you are more mature. So parents and children take note, you know, for those of you that are saying, "Oh, you know, the child is more like you or more like me, that can change across the lifespan." And if you're thinking about your parental lineage and wondering whether or not you quote unquote inherited some sort of trait from mother or from father, it can be, of course, both, or it can be just one or just the other. Which I think most parents tend to see and describe in their children from time to time. That's just like the father or that's just like the mother of friends. Right. But it's important to know that in this situation, their environment played no role. This was just whether it passed to the mother or the father.


Nature vs. Nurture; Epigenetics & Offspring (50:43)

It's not that something that happened to the mother or the father affected it. So this is slightly different. The question is now, can the environment change the heritutum to you? So it's very important to understand that there is a difference between nurture and nature. And this is very confusing. People are, it's a little subtle. So for example, people tell me, "I'm growing horses for many years. And I just know that this horse has a particular character. It's very different from the other horse." And so this is epigenetic inheritance. No. It could be just genetically determined, yes, this horse inherited a different set of genetic instructions. So it is different. It doesn't have to be about epigenetics. Epigenetic inheritance means that the environment of the parents somehow changed the children. And there are these two main barriers that are fear-as-bad, butternext, that we have to think what type of molecule and how they can be bridged. So one possibility is that it's really this limited number of chemical modifications that survive, which is about 10% or so. That could be very interesting. Not a small number. Not a small number, but perhaps. Perhaps. This is one possibility. The other possibility, there are other mechanisms. The situation now in humans is that it's just really unclear what transmit, if it can transmit, on which molecule does it. We'll talk later about other organisms where it is a lot more clear. But in humans and in members in general, there are many examples for environments that change the children. Whether you need to invoke an epigenetic mechanism to explain this phenomena, this is unclear. First of all, because it's hard to separate nurture from nurture. And second, because the mechanism is just not understood. So there are classic examples. For in humans, there were periods of famine, starvation in different places in the world. In the Netherlands, in China, in Russia, where people did huge epidemiological study to study the next generation, and so that the children of women who were starved during pregnancy are different. In many ways, they have different birth weight, glucose sensitivity, and also some neurological, higher changes of getting some neurological diseases. And this has been shown in very large studies. Is there ever an instance of which starvation or hardship of some kind, some challenge, a sensory challenge or survival-based challenge led to adaptive traits? Yes, there are in different organisms. It could be as a result of a trade-off. So there could be a downside as well. For example, there are two examples that come into mind. One of them is that if you stress male mice or rats, I don't remember. This is work of Isabel Mansui in the ETH in Switzerland. If you stress the males, you can do it in many different ways. I don't remember exactly how they did, but you can separate them from their mothers. You can do social defeat, all kinds of things. Then the next generations are less stressed. They show less anxiety. So they're threshold for stress is higher? Yes. However, I think they have memory deficits and other metabolic problems. Which may be an advantage for dealing with stress. It could be. I don't have any direct evidence that, but there's some simmering ideas that our ability to anchor our thoughts in the past, present or future seems very adaptive in certain contexts. In other contexts, it can keep us ruminating and not adaptively present to our current challenges. Another example is that nicotine exposure, this is the work of Oliver Handel from UMass. If I'm not mistaken, these are not my studies, but they improve the tolerance to exposure to similar drugs in the next generation. The interesting thing here is that it's very nonspecific. You treat them with nicotine, but then in the next generation, they're more tolerant to nicotine, but also to other things, cocaine. That makes sense to me because, obviously, nicotine activates the colonergic system, the dopaminergic system, epinephrine, etc. You can imagine that there's crossover because other drugs like cocaine and phetamine mainly target the catecholamine. In this particular study, if I remember correctly, they show that this happens with heritable effect, even if you use an antagonist to block the nicotine receptor. Wow. So it's something more about clearance of xenobiotics and hepatic functions that is transmitted and is very nonspecific. What I love about all the examples you've given today, and especially that one is, and I hope that people, if you're just listening, I'm smiling because biology is so complex. The obvious mechanism is rarely the one that's actually at play. People always ask, "Well, why? Why is it like this?" And I always say, "The one thing I know for sure is that I wasn't consulted with the design phase." And if anyone claims they were, then you definitely want to back away very fast. And there could be so many trade-offs. So for example, we studied, and also many other people studied, effects, these are in worms. We'll go deep into that in a second, but show that when you starve them, the next generations live longer. And this, I think, could be a trade-off with other things like fertility. So the next generations are more sick and less fertile, and perhaps because of that, they live longer. So it's not necessarily a good thing, right? I don't want to draw you off course, because this is magnificent what you're doing and, and, and, and, and, uh, it's playing out for us here. But to recall, there was a few years ago, it actually ended very tragically. It was an example. I think it was down in San Diego County. There was a cult of sorts that were interested in, um, living forever. And so they, uh, castrated, the male self-castrated themselves in the idea that somehow, um, maintaining some pre-pubescent state or reverting to a pseudo-pre-pubescent state would somehow extend longevity. The idea that, um, sexual behavior somehow limited lifespan. This has been an idea that's been thrown around in the kind of more wacky longevity communities. Um, they also shaved their heads. They also wore the same sneakers, but then they also all committed suicide, right? As the Hale-Bopp comet came through town. Um, but that's just, but one example of many cults aimed at sort of, um, that obviously was not life extension, that was life truncation, but, um, aimed at kind of eternal life for some sort of, uh, through caloric restriction. That's right. This, this cult also was very into the whole idea that by, through caloric restriction, we can live much longer, which, uh, may actually turn out to be true. I think it's still debated. Um, hence all the debate about intermittent fasting, et cetera. But also, uh, it is known that if you overeat, you shorten life. This is, this is clear. It's known that big bodied members of a species live far, um, shorter lives than the smaller members of a great day versus a chihuahua, for instance. So there is some, like, sort of shards of truths in all of these things, but I, I, it seems to me that the real question is like, what is the real mechanism and, and why would something like this exist? Right. And why questions are very dangerous in the hell, and you're right. Right. But, but very interesting goes on. And, and so, so when it comes to metabolic changes and nutrition, there are numerous examples where you either overfeed or start and get effect in the next generation. Many of the sometimes the effects contrast depending on the way you do this. Uh, again, these are none of, we don't do any of that in, in members, but people show that you're starving or overfeeding the mothers, all the fuddles changes the body weight of the, the next generation, and also the glucose tolerance and, and other and also a productive success.


Generational Epigenetic Inheritance (59:06)

And, and, and so the fact that there's an effect that there's something transmit this is clear. The question is, how miraculous is it, and whether you need new biology and epigenetics to explain it. What do I mean by that? If you affect the next generation, it doesn't necessarily has to go through the oocyte or the sperm and involves the epigenome. You change the metabolism of the animal as it develops, and obviously it will affect it. Okay, that when you, for example, a star, women that are pregnant, has happened during the famous starvation studies. The, the baby is already in utero, exposed directly to the environment. So it's not even a heritable effect. The baby is itself that affected. It's a direct effect. Very interesting, important and has many implications, and it will be separate from the genetics. You'll have to take it into account to understand what's going on. Doesn't require necessarily new biology, a new, new biology of inheritance. Not only is the embryo affected, the embryo while in utero already has jumpsets. So it's also the next generation is directly exposed. And you don't need any new biology necessarily to explain it. And it doesn't have, has to involve genetic epigenetics or epigenetics. It's clear to me that in the female fetus, the total number of eggs that she will someday produce and potentially have fertilized by sperm exist. But in males with a 60 day sperm cycle, leads me to the question, do fetal males, males as fetuses, living as fetuses in their moms already start producing sperm, or it's the primordial cells that give rise to sperm. So I'm not an expert, so I don't want to go into the details of exactly when in mammals. But yes, exposure of the mother also affects eventually the transmission of genetic information for the sperm's father. And there are also many examples of just stressing the father's affecting the sperm and affecting the next generation. There, if you go to the F2 generation, if you go two generations down the road, not to the kids, but to the grandkids, then it is a real epigenetic effect because you examine something that happens, although the next generation was never exposed to the original challenge. So when we say about epigenetic inheritance through the paternal lineage, through the founders, we talk about two generations, and when you go through the mother, it's three generations to talk about when you need to invoke some real epigenetic mechanism. And there, the evidence becomes much more scarce in men's. There are examples, more or less convincing. The field is evolving and improving a lot. So for example, now, to many people use the cutting edges to use IVS, in vitro fertilization, or transfer of embryos to make sure that you actually, it's the, it's the heritable information, not the environment, or that it goes through the germline. So this is something that is being done now. There are studies. You're talking about the three parent IVF, where they take the DNA from mom, the sperm from dad, and they take the DNA from mom and put it into a novel cytoplasm. And none is all you just, you just take the sperm and transfer it and fertilize it, the, the, an egg. So standard IVF? Yeah, standard IVF. Yeah. You can do it in many different ways, but this idea that you separate the environmental, the environment of the mother from the inheritance or the environment of the father, and to control and separate nature from the the environment becomes the culture dish. Yes. So, so the field is improving. People do experiments that have a higher ends or more replicates and better controlled. And there are some examples for effects that transfer and it depends on the environment. And it's not the only way to do it. And so, the way to do it is to make sure that the environment becomes the culture dish. Yes. Yeah. So, so the field is, is improving. People do experiment. It have a higher ends or more replicates and better controlled. And there are some examples for effects that transfer. And it depends who you ask whether people believe it or not. And many people do believe it and it depends on the community. There are strong resistance for many reasons. Some of them are justified. Some less justified and are part of the scientific process of and how things work because it's, it's, it's a new, it's challenging the dogma. So this is very interesting on its own. If you ask psychologist, many psychologists believe that there's heritable trauma and things like this. Population geneticists less so. Okay. So this really depends. And I think that we are just at a point in time where we don't really know whether it happens and to what extent. And we need bigger studies. Even if you think about normal, just genetic studies where people try to understand the genetic underpinning of complex traits. Like, like, anything that involves the brain pretty much. We now know that you need to study many, many, many people. So now this big genome wide association studies, big genetic studies involved hundreds of thousands of people. No one did an experiment like this for epigenetics. It's much more complicated because you need to also take into account the environment. I don't, I'm not even sure we know how to design such an experiment. It's very, very challenging. So, so the part of the resistance, the resistance to the idea is based on total theoretical grounds because of these barriers and because of the controversies. On the other hand, there's people really want to believe it. People really want to believe it. Because it sorts of gives your life meaning if you can change your biology through changing of your kids, through changing your biology sort of psychologically. I can understand why many people want this to happen. Even Schrodinger, the famous physicist, so he wrote a very important book in '44. So this was before the double helix and that's, it's called "What is Life". This is actually a book that was drove many physicists to establish molecular biology. It's very, very important. And he talks about the heritable material. It also talks about evolution. And he said, unfortunately, Lamarckism or inheritance of a quadrate is untenable. It doesn't happen. And he writes, this is very, very sad or unfortunate because unlike Darwinism or natural selection, which is gloomy, doesn't matter what you do, the next generation will be born based on the instruction in this experiment. And he says, "You can't influence it. Of course, you can give your kids money and education, but you can't biologically influence it." You can also, one thing I'm fascinated by for a number of reasons is partner selection. I mean, in some ways, we think, "Oh, we want to find someone who is kind." That does seem to be, by the way, the primary feature, at least in the data, tell us. We had David Bus on the podcast of how women select men that people are kind. And also resource potential. There's also beauty or aesthetic attractiveness in males and females, et cetera. Male, male, female, female, as the case may be, but in terms of reproductions, sperm, egg, male, female, obviously. So we're selecting for a number of traits, but presumably subconsciously, we are also selecting for a number of traits related to vigor. And the idea that if we were to have offspring with somebody, that those traits would be selected for. And we actually have a work on that in nematodes that I'll be happy to tell you about in a second after we... The dating in worms. The dating in worms. And where we understand the mechanism. And we'll go into that in a second, or in a few minutes after we dive into the worms. But yes, the original calculations of how population genetics work to simplify things and to do the math, so it will be as it was random mating. And of course, it doesn't work like that. So it's complicated things because we know. And there's research about potential capacity to somehow sense immune compatibility and things like this, which is, I don't know, I'm not an expert on that. And neither am I, but my understanding is that, of course, we're familiar with the other traits we select for, like, potential nurturing ability. Whether or not someone is reliable predicts something about their nurturing ability and offspring potentially. And you can draw lines between these things without any direct evidence, but they seem so logical, right, that somebody kind would also stick around or be honest and these kinds of things that it makes sense. But that one would be selecting for certain biological traits like immune function or some other form of robustness that we're not aware of is, I think, a fascinating, fascinating area of biology. So this is where the work in mammals stands. However, there's also one additional thing to mention, which is that on top of chemical modifications to the DNA and the proteins that condense the DNA, which are called histones. There are also other mechanisms that might transmit information, including transmission between generations of RNA. And there are different types of RNA, not just the RNA that we mentioned before, the messenger RNA, which encodes for the information for making important, but also other RNAs that regulate gene expression. And this is, and I think that in recent years, also in the mammalian field, RNA, as the molecule that has the potential to transmit information between generation, took center stage.


So I think this is the cutting edge. A lot more to understand and know, but RNA has a lot of potential for doing that, as we'll explain soon, but we have to go to worms first. And I'd like to just take a brief moment and thank one of our podcast sponsors, which is Inside Tracker. Inside Tracker is a personalized nutrition platform that analyzes data from your blood and DNA to help you better understand your body and help you reach your health goals. A long-been and believer in getting regular blood work done for the simple reason that blood work is the only way that you can monitor the markers such as hormone markers, lipids, metabolic factors, etc. that impact your immediate and long-term health. The major challenge with blood work, however, is that most of the time, it does not come back with any information about what to do in order to move the values for hormones, metabolic factors, lipids, etc. into the ranges that you want. With Inside Tracker, changing those values becomes very straightforward because it has a personalized dashboard that you can use to address the nutrition-based, behavior-based, supplement-based approaches that you can use in order to move those values into the ranges that are optimal for you, your vitality, and your longevity. Inside Tracker now includes a measurement of Apolipo Protein B, so-called APOB, in their ultimate plan. APOB is a key marker of cardiovascular health, and therefore there's extreme value to knowing your APOB levels.


Model Organisms, C. elegans (01:10:20)

If you'd like to try Inside Tracker, you can go to insidetracker.com/huberman to get 20% off any of Inside Tracker's plans. Again, that's insidetracker.com/huberman to get 20% off. Thank you for that incredible overview of genetics and RNA and epigenetics, and it was essentially a survey of this very interesting. On the face of a complex field, but you've simplified it a great deal for us. In our transition to talking about worms, I would like to plant a flag in the Huberman Lab podcast and say that what we are about to discuss is the first time that anyone on this podcast has discussed so-called model organisms. I may have mentioned a fly paper here or there, or a study on honeybees and caffeine and flower preference at one point, but typically that's done in passing and we quickly rotate to humans. I know that many, if not most of our listeners, are focused on humans and human biology and health, etc. But I cannot emphasize enough the importance of model organisms and the incredible degree to which they've informed us about human health, especially when it comes to very basic functions and cells. One could argue, "Okay, and there's been some debate telomeres in mice. Did that really lead to the same sort of data in humans?" Okay, there are those cases certainly, but model organisms are absolutely critical and have been and basically informed most of what we understand about human health. So before we start to go into the description about worms per se, could you just explain to a general audience what a model organism is? Right, they're not modeling, they're not posing for photographs, obviously. What that means and what some of the general model organisms are and why you've selected or elected to work on a particular type of worm to study these fascinating topics that there's zero question also take place in humans at some level. So it's a real pleasure and an honor to represent the model organisms here. I'm really happy just for that. It was worth it. Because as you said, model organisms are extremely important and we learned so much about biology through them. The model organisms mean that it's an organism that many people work on, so there's a community of people that work on. People work study many types of organisms, but not around every organism. There's a huge community of researchers that combine sources to create all the resources and the tools and understanding that accumulate. There are just a handful of model organisms in the short history of the field of biology. It's not so long. We learned about every aspect of biology through them, including many important diseases, human diseases. These are E. coli bacteria, phage, which is a virus of bacteria, flies, worms, they're called theeligants nematotes, this is what we studied in the lab. Fish, which are called the bebophish. It's dannio dannio. Of course, there are also model organisms, other animals. And also plants, important plants, the most important studies, one is the rabi doppies. And perhaps less so nowadays, but non-human primates, macaque monkeys, marmoset, squirrel monkeys. These, I don't know exactly how to definition this, but emerging model organisms. There are many model organisms that are emerging and there are communities that are formed, including also around the planaria that we mentioned before, this flatworm that regenerates. This is a great model for studying regenerations. If we could develop new heads, it would be incredible. And we can learn from these organisms. And the reason that we can learn a lot also about humans by studying these animals is that we all evolved from the same ancestor. So we shared a lot of our functions with them, and also a lot of our genes. Sea elegans, and they have different model organisms have different advantages that serve us. They sometimes have some things that are much more apparent in them that we can study. For example, learning and memory was largely studied in the beginning in a snail, a plesia, where many of the discoveries were made because it has big noi ones that you can easily study and examine. And yes, snails learn. Yes, they learn. Even sea elegans, these nematodes that we study learn, and they are much simpler than another important reason to study them. Of course, you can actually experiment on them. We can't do this to humans, the things that we do to these animals. And we can change their genes to all kinds of things for them. And in some cases, I think you're going to tell us, for instance, in sea elegans in particular, the presence of particular cell types is so stereotyped. That you can look at several different worms, and the community of people that study sea elegans is literally numbered and named each neuron, so that two laboratories on opposite sides of the world can publish papers on the same neuron, knowing that it's the same neuron in the two different laboratories. Something that is extremely hard to do in any mammalian model, a mouse or certainly in humans, and has posed huge challenges that give great advantages to studies of things like sea elegans. Yes, so sea elegans, this is the star now of what we study. These are nematodes, small worms, round worms, that are just one millimeter long, so you can't see them with a naked eye. You have to look under the scope. Where do they live in the natural world? So, they used to call them soil nematode, but this is not really true. They are in many places, but they are mostly in rotten fruits and leaves. And you can find them in the ground as well, but you can also find them, and they are free-living so they're not parasites, but you can sometimes also find them in snails. But the best way to isolate them is from rotten fruits. I like the idea that they're not parasites. I'm one of these people that gets a little squeamish about the notion of parasites. Yes, so they're not parasites, they're really fun to handle because they're so small and easy. You just grow them on plates with eggar and E. coli bacteria, this is what they eat in the lab. You can just pick them with a smaller wire pick, move them around and change their genes and do many things to them. But they have many advantages for neuroscience and for studying inheritance. As you mentioned, they have always a certain number of cells in the body. So, a ceiling and stematode always says 959 cells in its body. That's it. Not 968. 959. And out of which 302 are neurons, always 302. There's a huge debate now over Twitter on whether it's 302 or 300. I mean, I don't want to get into trouble, okay? But people take this very, very hard. I think it's 302, but let's not get into it because I'll get into trouble. Well, we can equilibrate all things here by, you say 302, granted you're far more informed in this model organism than I am or ever will be. I'll say 300. And then we were balanced in terms of partisan politics and the C elegans community. Perfect. So, and it's always the same. And each neuron has a name, like you said. And not only does every neuron has a name, many of them, we know what they do. So, there's a few cells that are sensory neurons that sense particular chemicals. In certain situations, we know that a chemical will be sent just by one neuron. There are other motor neurons and internal neurons and all of that. We know how many dopamine neurons there and serotonin neurons. And we know them all by their name. Not only that, we know how they are connected to one another. We have a map, a connectome, since the 80s, like a subway map, that tells us which neuron talks with the other neurons. And it is the same. It was used to think, people thought that it was exactly the same between genetically identical worms. Now we know that there are slight differences, but by and large, it is the same. And we have a map, a roadmap that we can use to study. The so-called connectome. The connectome. Not only that, the worms are transparent. So, we can actually see the neurons fire using particular tools. And we can activate genes and silage genes using optogenetics. Like, we'll discuss here on the podcast. We can make the worms go forward or backward or lay a neck by shining different waves of light on them. And so, we have very powerful tools for manipulating the brain. On top of that, we have great understanding of the genetics of the genome. This is, C.E.L.A.N. is the first animal to have its genome sequenced, before humans. For that, of course, there were bacteria. And we know that each worm produces each mother, produces about 250 babies, which are almost genetically identical. So, we know where we grow them. The environment is very controlled. So, we grow them in the plate with just bacteria. So, we can easily separate with nature and nurture. And one thing that I wonder about often is generation time. Even though mice are not humans, mice have certain advantages because they're mammalian species. You can't do all the magnificent things that you can do and see all the genes in mice. But one major issue with mice is that the generation time is somewhat long. You pair two mice, they mate, you get a mouse or litter of mice 21 days later. It might seem like, "Okay, that's only 21 days or so." But if you are a graduate student or a postdoc trying to do a project, I mean, that can extend the time to do experiments out three or four years compared to what you could do in C.E.L.A.L.A.N. You're absolutely right. This is one of the major advantages. The generation time in C.E.L.A.N. is three days. So, you can do hundreds of worm generations in one PhD. This is very important. Not only that, every worm will produce hundreds of progenies, so you will have that are genetically identical, so you will have great statistics for your experiment. And the worms probably don't mind living on these agar plates, munching away on E. coli. It's the good life. It's questionable whether or not mice or certainly, listen, I'm a proponent of well controlled over, and as long as there's oversight, animal research, it's necessary for the development of treatments of diseases that hinder humans. But it is always a little bit of a kind of a cringin go kind of thing when you're dealing with mammals that are living so far outside their natural environment. I'd be lying if I didn't say that it gets to you after a while, and if it doesn't get to you, you kind of have to wonder about your own psyche of it. Right. I also think that this is important, but for me, it's much easier to work on worms. I don't have to feel bad about it. Yeah, they're happy. If they're happy, and you also, I mean, if a worm dies, it's less painful to the human than if an other more sensitive animal said. Yeah, I would argue. Yes, I agree.


C. Elegans & The Inheritance Of Acquired Traits

C. elegans & Inheritance of Acquired Traits, Small RNAs (01:21:50)

So, yes, there are many advantages for studying sea elephants. And in the worm, we now have very obvious and clear cut proof that there is inheritance of acquired traits. So much so that I don't think that anyone pretty much in the epigenetic field argues against it. Well, and in large part, thanks to you and the work you've done. So, could you tell us what was the first experiment that you did on sea elegance that confirmed for you that there is inheritance of acquired traits? Because, of course, the best experiments and experimenters always set out to disprove their hypothesis. And when the hypothesis survives, despite all the control experiments and poking and prodding attempts to contradict oneself, then it's considered a victory. But it's one that we all have to be very cautious about enjoying because of the tendency to want our hypotheses to be true. So, what was the first experiment where you were convinced that inheritance of acquired traits is real? The first experiment I did was in my postdoc, which I did with Oliver Hobart in the University of Columbia. We set to test whether worms can produce transgenerational, multiple generations resistance to viruses. Oh, wow, this is a very pertinent topic. Yes, which is a relevant day. And worms, these worms, don't have dedicated immune cells like we do. They don't have T cells or B cells. They defend themselves from viruses very efficiently using RNA. So, in fact, when we started these experiments, there wasn't any natural virus that was known to infect the elegance, which is amazing, because viruses are very good as we all experience now in infecting. And the worms are resistant to viruses because of RNA molecules, short RNA molecules that destroy viruses. And these are called smaller RNAs. Now we need to discuss them before I explain my experiment. In 2006, two researchers that were studying C. elegans, Andrew Fier and Craig Maylog had the Nobel Prize for showing that there is a mechanism that is known to be used in the genome. There is a mechanism that regulates genes that happens for smaller RNAs. What they've shown is that if you inject the worms with RNA molecules, which are double-stranded, they lead to the shot of the genes that match in sequence to this RNA. So, sort of like taking the specific instructions for the coffee table from your IKEA handbook, and you insert a copy of that into the book, and in doing so, you prevent the expression of sort of erase the original page. Perfect explanation. Perfect explanation. And they found that double-stranded RNA RNA that has two strands is what starts the response, leading to the production of small RNA molecules, which are the ones that actually find the messenger RNA and leads to destruction, silences, so you don't get proteins in the end. For that, they got the Nobel Prize. After people found that this is conserved in many organisms, including humans, and now there are now drugs. This was only in 2006, the Nobel Prize. The paper was published in 1998. There are now drugs that use this mechanism, also in humans. And I'll just interject and say that not only is it a recent discovery and an incredibly important one, but Andy Fire and Craig Miller are also really nice people. They just happen to be very nice people. And Craig Miller is an excellent, I think he's a kite surfer. The only time I met him in person was at a meeting in hand, a black eye, and I thought, "Okay, wow, I guess he's also a pugilist or something." But it turns out he had done that kite surfing. So scientists actually do things other than go to the laboratory, Nobel Prize-winning scientist, that is. Okay, I'll let you continue. Thanks for allowing for that introduction.


RNA Interference, C. elegans & Virus Immunity (01:26:02)

Yeah, incredible scientists. There were also studies in many organisms on the mechanisms of how this happens. It is called RNA interference. RNA interferes in the expression of a gene in the function of a gene. And it's also called gene silencing because these RNAs enforce the silencing of genes. Instead of the genes being expressed, they are silenced and you don't manage that stuff function. Okay? Already in the first paper that they published about this, where they've shown the double strand RNAs, which leads to the silencing of the control, they've shown two very important things. One of them is that if you inject the worms with double strand RNA, you don't only see the action in the cell that you injected or in the tissue that you injected, but you see it all over the worm's body. It spreads. It wasn't exactly clear what spreads, but it was clear that it spreads. You see the silencing all over the body. This includes also the germ cells. So if you inject the double strand RNA just to somatic cells, even to the head, you will get also the effect in the germ cells and in the next generation. Okay? In the immediate cogent in the F1 generation, the kids. So this was really clear proof that this is inherited. However, this is just one generation. Okay? In this original study. Later, the gene, something which will immediately remind you what I told you about with planaria, that you can just take worms and feed them on bacteria that produce this double strand RNA. And that the double strand and the silencing would move from the site of ingestion from the gut, where the bacteria are eaten, to the rest of the body, and also to the next generation. Okay? So before we left, when I mentioned this cannibalistic experiments of McConnell with the planaria, and now you see that it can happen, and this is not controversial at all. This is being done routinely every day by any cell against biologists in the world. This has been replicated a million times. Not only that, you can also feed planaria, these other worms with RNA. You can just put it in chopped liver and let them eat it, and again, this will sign in just throughout the body. Okay? Wild. Okay? And this is what we do routinely. We always, when we use this technique to see what genes do. If we want to see whether a particular gene is important for a certain behavior or a certain something, the way to study is to neutralize the gene activity. And we do it by just introducing the worms with double strand RNA that correspond in sequence, that match in sequence this gene. This will lead to the silencing, this activate the gene's activity, and then we, and if the effect stops, we know this gene is involved in the function. And we never want to just examine one worm. So we feed the mother with double strand RNA, and then we examine all of its children, so we can have the statistics over hundreds of worms, or thousands of worms. So this is validated and not controversial in all, and totally routine. Is it fair to say that McConnell's experiments have chopped blending up these worms, really graphic image, blending up these worms and then feeding them to other worms, planaria, that those experiments can, yes, be explained by double stranded RNA, which, and through RNA, interferor. Eventually, it hasn't been done yet. We are working on it in my lab now in collaboration with other labs, but it wasn't published, but yes, this could be the explanation. So, fire and mellow did these experiments, some other people did these experiments. When I started my work, I wanted to see whether in addition to artificial double stranded RNA, some natural traits can also transmit co-generations because of RNA, because smaller than this. Right, because injecting RNA I, or in short, interfering RNAs, that is, or putting worms into an environment with an abundance of inhibitory RNAs as an experiment is very different than worms experiencing something and then passing on that acquired trait to their offspring. It's a world apart, in my opinion, because one is an extreme manipulation that illustrates an underlying principle, the other is something that, in theory, occurs in the passage of generations, just naturally. We're going from the lesser deficient to the more artificial. The advantages, just like with model organisms, that the more artificial it is, the easier it is to, you know exactly what you did. Just now introduce one factor and you can follow the result. So this is always the trait. What I did was, in all of us lab, is to see whether the magic, part of the magic for the worms resistance to viruses is the capacity to transmit information in the form of RNA molecules, inhibitory RNA molecules, to the next generations. And it has been shown before in C. elegans that the worms resist viruses using this mechanism, the smaller anase. In fact, this is probably the reason that these smaller anase evolved in the first place to get rid of viruses and other parasitic genomic elements. And this is a mechanism to fight them. And what I did is a very simple experiment. I took worms and I infected them with the virus. When you do this, this also has been shown in the past. The worms destroy the virus. Okay? We demonstrated this very clearly using a fluorescent virus. So if the virus replicates successfully, the worms just turns green. And if the virus is destroyed, the worms stay black. This is very simple. It's a clear cut off. It's not, you don't examine the worm and ask whether it feels good. You just see this green light. It's a very simple, very simple, very simple, very simple, very simple, very simple. And so we took worms. We infected them with the fluorescent virus. They destroyed. This also has been done in the past. But then what we did is we neutralized the machinery that makes smaller anase in the descendants of the worms. And then we started on their own because they just don't have the genes that you need to make this smaller anase. Okay? And then we asked, what will happen will affect these worms with the valves? Will they be green or black? They can't make their own smaller anase. So they can't protect themselves on their own. The only way for them to stay black for them not having the valves replicate is if they inherit the smaller anase from the pet. That's exactly what happens. All the worms project, although they don't have the gene that is needed for making the smaller anase are black. They signed the valves. And this also continues for additional generations. So the parent worms effectively put something into the genetic instructions of the offspring that would afford them, let's call it an advantage in this case. But afford them an advantage if they were to be confronted with the same thing that the parents were. And we know exactly what these advantages are small RNAs that match the viral genome. Then just chop up the virus in the next generation. And we can identify these smaller anase in the inhibitory organase in the descendants, although they don't have the machinery to make it. Just because we can identify them by sequencing. By RNA sequencing, which is like DNA sequencing, you actually get the actual sequence of the RNA molecules. And we can see that they correspond to the valves and they inherit the smaller anase only if their parents were infected with them. So there's specificity there? There's specificity. Yeah, it's not some just general resilience passage.


RNA Amplification, Multi-Generational Effects (01:34:13)

Right. I have to be careful in drawing an analogy that isn't correct. And I want to acknowledge that what I'm about to say with certainty cannot be entirely correct. But the analogy that comes to mind in mammals is this idea that if one generation is stressed, that their offspring may, in some cases, have a higher stress threshold, a resilience to stress. I could imagine why that would be advantageous. My parents have a hard life. They have offspring and they want their children to have a higher threshold of stress because stress can inhibit reproduction, etc. And I always say, you know, at the end of the day and at the end of life, evolution is about the offspring, not about the parents. And every species pretty much seems to want to make more of itself and protect its young one way or another, either through nature or through nurture. This is a nature-based protection of young. Is it fair to say that in the mammalian experiment with a passage of stress resilience, that it could be RNA-based, that that would be, perhaps, set some new threshold on glucocorticoid production? Here, I'm speculating, and I want to highlight that I'm speculating. I'm speculating with a reason, which is I think for people that are hearing about this in worms, you've done a beautiful job of playing out why model organisms are really important. But to think about how this may operate in the passage of human generations, I think is a reasonable thing to entertain. Right, and it is true that also in mammals, now RNAs and smaller RNAs are lending candidates for something that could mediate the transmission of stress protection or also harmful effects to transmittive generation. Perhaps RNA-duit. However, in worms, the RNAs have one more trick that we don't know the equivalent in mammals yet. This is something very crucial that we showed in that particular paper, in the first paper. Which is? So, the effect that I described, this transmission of resistance to viruses, through these RNAs, doesn't only affect the next generation. It also affects multiple additional generations. So it gets passed. It gets passed and you have to ask yourself, how does it get diluted? Why is it diluted? Because everyone produces 250 babies. So you dilute by 250. And if something is diluted for four generations, so it's 250 times two hundred and fifty. After four generations, it's a dilution of four billions. Completely on my own topic would never work. There's nothing left. The secret of these worms is that they have a machinery for amplifying the smaller RNAs in every generation. This is called RNA-dependent RNA polymerase. It's a complex which uses the RNA to find, and once it finds the messenger RNA, it just creates many, many, many smaller RNAs. So they don't get diluted and they pass on for additional generations. And this is the trick. We later also identify genes that regulate for how long an effect would last. Otherwise, if in the beginning we ask, how does it stop after one generation, now we have to ask, why does it last forever? And it doesn't. Typically, we see that the responses last not only with the viruses, but also with other traits for a few generations, three to five generations. We found genes that function as a sort of a clock that times the duration of the inheritance. What sorts of genes are those? So we call these genes "motec genes." I don't know how is your Hebrew, but "motec" means sweetheart in Hebrew. But the acronym is "Modified Transgenerational Epigenetic Genetics." There are different types of genes like that. And some of them, if you mutate, if you disrupt their function, now the effect will transmit stably for hundreds of generations. It will never stop. Because their role is to stop the inheritance from going from just, you don't want to carry over something forever. Otherwise, it will no longer fit the environment of the parents and you'll be prepared for the wrong things. So this is important. What type of genes are they? One gene that we studied, it's called "metoo." It's actually a gene that functions in methylation of the proteins that condense the DNA. So this is...


Response Duration & Environment (01:38:41)

But when there are other genes that affect also production of smaller RNAs. Is there some mechanism that controls the duration of passage in a way that logically links up with the lifespan of the organism? So for instance, I knew my grandparents met them. I did not ever meet my great-grandparents. And I certainly didn't meet my great-great-grandparents. I could imagine that my great-great-grandparents, or my great-grandparents, experienced certain things that were passed into their children and perhaps into their children. But it seems reasonable, given that humans live somewhere between zero and a hundred years. Typically what now? Is that the typical lifespan? More or less? Okay. If I were going to design the system, and again, I was not consult the design phase, I would want an adaptive trait to be passed for two generations because given how long our species lives, and certainly given the way the world looks now as opposed to the turn of the previous century or the turn of the previous century, different stressors, different life environments. And what I would want to pass on to my offspring, I can basically hedge pretty well. I can place a good bet on the next hundred years, maybe the next 200, but I don't have the foggiest clue what the world is going to look like in 300 years. Is this what I'm saying, make any sense whatsoever? It makes a lot of sense. And really, we need to talk about two things in response to this question. First of all, yes, you can imagine that the reason that the worms in herds typically for three to five generations is that this is relevant. This is relevant to something that happened in their environment. For example, we also show that when you starve the worms, it affects the next generations again for a few generations. Which itself is amazing. I just want to highlight that. You can imagine next generation, sort of like a genetic version of be careful kids, but I'm going to give you this extra lunch pack in your genome that protects you against the possibility of starvation. But it's also saying, and we're you to have kids. Right. They have it also. So I have to just make a disclaimer that we don't know that necessarily it's adaptive. It could also be damaged. As I said, when you starve them, the next generations live longer, but this could be a trade off for fertility or something. So other labs is also also shown following our work that if you starve the worms, the next generations are also more resistant to house health starvation. This sounds, this is not my artwork, but this sounds adaptive. But when whenever you're talking about adaptation, you have to see it in the context of evolution. There's also this famous saying, nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. And so it's very hard to say without doing the lab evolution experiments, we actually see who wins the ones that inherit or the ones that don't have, who takes over. Otherwise, it's hard to talk about whether it's adaptive or not. But when it comes to the duration of the response, yes, it could be programmed to fit something. For example, if you're talking about starvation, worms transition between periods of starvation and periods where they have a lot of food. So let's say they sign an apple. For a few generations, they will consume the apple and then they will be starved for a while. Perhaps this is the number of generations that takes them to finish an apple. Or perhaps there are other responses also to higher temperatures. If you grow worms in higher temperatures, the offerings are different. They change how they mate. That's what I alluded to before. We're going to get back to this. Because it relates to cold exposure, which many listeners are interested in. Perhaps it is somehow correlated with the cycle of the year. But to tell you the truth, I don't know. As I said, we go from the more artificial to the less artificial. If double-strandary nature synthetic RNA is the most artificial, starvation is more natural, but it's not starvation. The real context of the world in a real apple is a plate with or without equally bacteria. It's not an apple on a tree exposed to the elements with other worms with bacteria with all kinds of complications. And it could be that we will see different durations of heritable effects. The more natural we go, it's just much less controllable and hard to do. And again, when we're talking about humans, part of the argument where people, these believers, it's not about say. The critics say that this wouldn't happen in humans if they say, the worms generation time is just three days. The chances that the parents' environment will match, the children's environment is very high. Because there's not a lot of time for the environment to change it, plus they can't go very far. There are many examples of epigenetic inheritance in plants. This is a big field where they're very established proof for inheritance of a quiet late. For epigenetic inheritance, I'll be more careful. Epigenetic inheritance is more loaded than. In plants, it also happens. And there you also say these are all sesal organisms. They can't run away, so the environment is more constant. Ideas, maybe just a quick example that I've heard before, tell me if I'm wrong, I very well maybe. For instance, a particular species of plant that grows a straight, maybe slightly bended stalk might be exposed to some environment where in order to capture enough sunlight and other nutrients, might need to grow in a corkscrew form. The corkscrew form can be inherited several generations. This is an example that I don't know, but perhaps it's something like that. I seem to, someone will tell, trust me, the one thing we know about podcasting and YouTube is someone will tell us in the comments. So, Ben, please do, we invite that. Right, but there's a long history of epigenetic inheritance studies in plants with excellent studies, well-controlled showing that it happens also there. So this is very clear. When it comes to humans, you could say, maybe my kids will go off to live in a different continent, and they will be on the computer every day, and everything will be different, so it makes less sense to prepare them for the same hardships that I experienced. However, this in my opinion, this argument comes a lot. It's not the best argument because it depends on the scale of how you look at things. We experience, we meet, for example, I'm not saying that this is inherited, but in humans, but we experience the same pathogens and the same viruses all the time. Perhaps it's worse preparing for that. Again, I'm not saying that it happens, but it depends on the scale. Well, what you're describing makes perfect sense, and I do want to acknowledge these critics, whoever they may be. I do have the advantage that I don't work in this exact field, and so I'm happy to stand toe-to-toe with those critics now and say that at least in terms of an inheritance of reactions or adaptive or maladaptive traits to stress or to reward. You talked about nicotine before, a passage of response to drugs of different kinds, not being specific to nicotine. It was a more general passage of some sort of information related to reactions to chemicals present in nicotine but other drugs. I have long been irritated and a little bit tickled by the fact that people say, "Oh, we have this system for stress that was really designed to keep us safe from lions and saber-tooth tigers." Sure, but the hallmark of the stress system is that it generalizes. I mean, if I get a troubling text message or if I suddenly see a dark figure in the hallway when I go to the bathroom at night that I don't recognize, both of those have the same generic response, which is the deployment of adrenaline in both brain and body changes in the optics of the eyes, quickening of the heart rate. Stress is by design generic. One could imagine that a passage of some sort of stress resilience or a maladaptive passage to stress would be also somewhat generic, and that's actually advantageous overall. Same thing with the reward system. We essentially have one or two chemical systems of reward. I mean, there's the opioid system and there's a cannabinoid system, but in large part, anticipation and reward is governed by the dopamine circuits. And anticipation and reward of an ice cream cone for a kid is the same neural circuitry that's going to be repurposed when they get to reproductive age and they are anticipating creating children with their mate and assuming they want to do that, the dopamine energy system is going to be engaged. So ice cream, sex, stress to weather, stress to famine, the biology of these more modal systems, and especially in the nervous system are, again, I have to be careful with the words by design. Art certainly generic. And so I don't see the need for so for immense specificity. I mean, it's not like where COVID just happened. So could you imagine that there's the passage of a COVID-19 specific resilience? No, I think what would probably be passed along would be some sort of, if it does occur, would be some sort of resilience to viruses more generally, and that would be advantageous. Right.


Generational Memory Transmission, RNA (01:47:50)

So I agree, and this opens the question of what is the bandwidth of inheritance? How specific can it be that it makes sense for it to be specific? And in the case of CLGANs, the response can be very specific through this inheritance of RNAs, which are just sequence specific, they don't regulate, they control one particular gene. In other cases, it could be a very general response. And it's very interesting to think about it when we talk about inheritance of memories, which is the most interesting thing we could imagine. Can brain activity of some sort transmit, at least in these words? I said, Noah, I said this is a claim multiple times in members. We don't know, times will tell, in words we know a lot. So can worms transmit brain activity to do they have the specificity to do it? Okay. Before I'll say that, I'll just say that we already learned a lot about the mechanisms that shuttle the RNAs between generations. We know about genes that are needed just for that. About worms would be perfectly okay, but just don't have the capacity to transfer the RNAs to the next generations. We know about genes that will make the responses longer or shorter. We know about genes that prevent the transfer of RNA between different issues. About genes that make certain small RNAs. So we know a lot about that. And then the question arises, we can finally ask, can memory transfer between generations? I think that first of all, we need to define memory for this. And the broadest definition would be any change in your behavior because of what happened in the past. Or in your response because what happened in the past or because of your history. The more interesting part of course is to talk about memories that are encoded in the brain. And the reason is that the brain is capable of holding much more specific and elaborate memories. I think that any tissues that transmit RNA to the next generation and affect the next generation is interesting. The gut muscles everything. But the brain can synthesize information about the environment and about internal states and can also think ahead. And the most provocative thing you can say is that you could plan somehow the fate of your nation using your brain after taking many things into the code. Without talking to them. Without talking. And then instruction. So again, we go back to this instruction manual. It's like writing something into the instruction manual based on your own experience. Right. And can it happen? And what is the bandwidth? Can we transfer specific things? And then I have to agree with you that I would imagine that what can transfer and I could be wrong is a general something sensitivity. You can make the analogy to being inflamed or not. It's hypersensitive to pathogens. Hyper vigil. But it can also be something very specific. Now we have to understand that the brain uses a different language than the language of inheritance. The brain, the way we normally think about the brain is that it keeps information in sign ups in the connection between different neurons. And then you make some connection stronger and some other connections weaker. And you wire the nervous system in a different way. The information in the brain is synoptic. It is in the connection. On the other hand, heredible information of any sort has to go through a bottleneck of one cell. The fertilized egg. Because we all start from just one cell. So it cannot be in the connections because this cell doesn't have any connections with other cells. So heredible information has to be molecular. It has to be inside this one cell. So the question is can you or do you translate the information, this free destructure information of synoptic sign ups and the connection between brains in the architecture of the brain? Can you somehow translate it to heredible information to a molecular form? It's an incredibly important and deep question. It brings to mind something that was once told to me which as soon as I heard it was obvious but was very important in formulating my understanding of biology which is that a map is just the transformation of one set of points into another set of points. So a map of the world essentially is just you take what's been drawn out in terms of the architecture and the coastlines etc. and divisions between states and you transfer that to an electronic map or a piece of paper. It seems so obvious it's sort of a duh why are we talking about this. But just to make sure that people understand what you're really talking about is let's say the memory and I have a very distinct memory for my childhood phone number. Phone number doesn't exist anymore. And I won't give it out because then some other person might get you with repeated calls. But in any case I remember it. It's totally useless information but it lives in my neocortex or my hippocampus or somewhere as a series of connections between neurons at the locations as you call synapses. Would my grandchildren know that phone number? There's no reason to know. Would my children know that unless there was some adaptive reason or some other reason for them to know and this passage of acquired traits. And what you're saying is in order for that to happen there has to be a transformation of the neural circuit literally the wiring of neuron ABCD that relates and carries the information of that number. So we're going to get into the kind of nucleotide sequences that are contained in DNA or patterns of methylation or RNA more likely. So it's the transformation of one set of points in physical space to a translation of points in genetic space. Right. And then we have many problems. First of all we don't know a mechanism to translate between the two different languages, the language of the brain and the language of the head. We are not familiar with a mechanism like that. The next generation, if it's not a worm, if it's a mammal, would have a different brain. Even if it was genetically identical to the parent, the wiring of the brain and the particular neural circuits will be different. This is true for twins. It will always be true because it depends because it's partially random and it depends on the environment even if you have the same genetic instructions. So let's say you somehow had a mechanism, a miracle mechanism to take the 3D information and translate it to the language of inheritance. You would then in the next generation have to translate it again to the brain, although it is different. This sounds very unlikely. I'm playing a trick on you now. I believe it. I'm easy to trick. But if this is how it happened or if this was required, it could never happen in my opinion. Which means, and I still think, that there are certain memories that cannot transfer trans generation and this complex. Things that you learn about the environment that are arbitrary. None of our listeners' kids will remember this conversation. No way. This is impossible. Unless they're listening with them. There are some families or parents that tell them they're listening. But it cannot transmit because it's random and these are connections that are arbitrary. So this seems to be a limitation of what can transfer. On the other hand, so perhaps more general things could pass. These types of things I doubt take it as. However, you can nevertheless imagine that some things that are very specific, some memories that are very very specific, could nevertheless transmit from the brain after learning to the next generation. I'll give you an example. You can teach worms. Even if they have just 300 to neurons, you can teach them simple things about the world. For example, you can take an odour that they're worms like. The worms have thousands of odour antleceptals and they can recognize many, many molecules. They can smell them so they can find food or avoid enemies. You can take a food that the worms like and pare it to something bad like starvation. And then the worms will learn to dislike this odour. We don't know that this learning involves necessarily changing in the strength of sign-upsets. It's a possibility. But it doesn't have to be the case. It could be that just the receptor for this particular odour is being removed. And this is how they lay. Now they won't have the receptor, they won't smell, they won't like the odour. This is a possibility. This type of thing you can perhaps, not that anyone has showed it convincingly, transmits to the next generation because all it would take is an RNA that will control this particular receptor. So this is a possibility. People have shown things like that, not in C elegans, but people have shown things like this in mammals. They said that you learn certain things and then just in the next generation, that's a particular receptor would be metallated or would change and this would transmit the response. And on the one hand, it could be true. On the other hand, you need to understand they'll need to prove, and this wasn't done convincingly enough yet, how exactly does the information transfer from the brain to the germ cells and then in the next generation from the germ cells back to the brain to where the receptor needs to operate. And this is a challenge, this is the current state of the field that this is something that needs to be proven. What we did in C elegans is we showed that certain, that the brain can communicate with the next generation using smaller RNAs. And the disc can change behaviour and it doesn't require any translating between any language. It is very simple. What we've shown is that if you take a worm and you change the production of smaller RNAs just in its brain, in the next generations, their behaviour will be different, even though you don't mess with their brains. This is a paper that we published in 2019 in Cell. We showed that you just manipulate the production of endogenous natural RNAs in the worms brain that are always made, that you change the amount, and this changes the capacity of the worms in the next generation to find food. Not only in one generation, but three generations down the road. And the way that it works is that pertuiting the production of these smaller RNAs in the brain affects in the end the expression of a gene in the germline. One gene is called Sage2, don't know how it works, but we can do all kinds of controls and manipulate activity of the gene in C.L.T. is also affecting behaviour. And this gene works in the germ cells. The information needs to go from the brain to the germ cells. It doesn't need to go back from the germ cells to the brain to affect behaviour. And this depends, we know that this is a true epigenetic effect because it goes on for multiple generations and also because it requires the machinery that transfers RNAs between generations. If you don't have that protein that physically carries the RNA between generations, it doesn't happen. So it has to be RNA. It has to be RNA. And we can actually, we can also find the RNAs in the next generation to change. We sequence the actual RNAs, the change in the next generation. You mentioned that you don't know what Sage, this gene Sage does, but is it reasonable to assume that it does something in the context of the nervous system or that's unclear as well?


Germ Cells & Behavior; Body Cues (01:59:36)

It is possible, it is possible, but we have reasons to believe or experiments to show, although there could be alternative explanations, that it functions through the germline. Now you may ask, how can you affect behaviour just by changing the germ cells, right? Well, it would have to change the germ cells in very specific ways because as people probably recall, the germline, the germ cells are where the inheritable information is contained. But you can imagine, for instance, adjusting the gain or sensitivity rather on some sort of sensory foraging system, right? Right. And the interesting thing is it again can be quite unspecific. So it sounds weird that you change germ cells and it changes behaviour, it's permanent. But if you think about it, it is trivial. If you castrate a dog, it behaves differently, right? Sadly, yeah, I did that to my dog and I ended up putting him on testosterone therapy later and it brought him back. Yeah. Just as a side. As an aside. This is because the germ cells affect the soma, including the brain, in many ways by secreting certain chemicals. And also because the other cells develop from the germ cells. So some information could be transmitted over development or the course of development could be altered because of changes that occur in the germ cells. And for example, in members, one of the explanations for how heritable information transmits and it just affects something very own in development. I told you that the secret to worms in here is that they have the capacity to amplify these small RNAs all the time. This is what keeps it going and prevents the dilution. In members, we don't know of such an amplification mechanism. So you ask, how can a little bit of RNA or something without amplifying affect them in the entire organism? And it could be that you just perturb something in the very beginning. And you just have a few cells, or even in the placenta that develops in pregnancy. And these later throws everything off. And because of the US, you have many problems, metabolies and so on. And this is called the, it's an idea of the developmental origin of health and disease. Many of the functions occur early on in development. So you raise a number of incredibly fascinating aspects to this. I do have a question about one particular aspect. And feel free to pass on this for a future episode if it's going to take us too far off track. But something you said, it really captured my attention, although I was listening to all of it, which is that the germ cells, so in the case of males, it's going to be sperm and in the case of females, it can be eggs. Something perhaps not coincidental about those cells and the environment that they live in is that, yes, they contain the genetic information and pass it off spring. Of course, you explain how that works. But also it's a, those cells live in a region that is rich with hormones that can be secreted and in fact are secreted and through so-called endocrine signaling communicate with other cells, not just at the level of receptors on their surface, but also can enter the genomes of those cells and modify those cells. In other words, it seems to me that the microenvironment of the germ cells, the testes and the ovaries, are rich with information, not just for the passage to next generations, but also for all the, as you said, all the somatic cells of the body, they're telling the somatic cells of the body what to do and what to become. And the best example I can think about this would be puberty. I mean, I would argue that the, that one of the greatest rates of aging and transitions we go through in life is from puberty. I mean, a child becomes a very different person after puberty. They look at the world differently. They think about it differently. It's not just about the growth of the hair and the jaw and the atom's apple and breasts and so on. It's a transformation of the somatic cells from the same microenvironment that the DNA containing cells reside. Right. So once you think about it like this, it becomes obvious that just by affecting the germ cells, you can affect the rest of the body. And in theologians, they are experiment to show it very clearly. So for example, if you take, just take worms and prevent sperm production, it changes the capacity to smell. These are experiments done by others, which is obviously a brain function. And in a castrated dog, you're not just eliminating the possibility of transfer of DNA information to subsequent generations. You're also limiting the communication of the, yeah, without question, my bulldog, Costello, changed after castration. And it was a wonderful dog, but at some point developed some health issues. The introduction of a small amount of testosterone every other day. And then changed him fundamentally. In that case, for the better, back to a version of himself that I had only observed earlier, but also a different version of the same dog. And no, he wasn't humping everything. Maybe the occasional knee, particular people who has names I won't mention. But it was absolutely clear that the hormone was not just taking a system and amplifying it. It was actually modifying the system. So anyway, I just wanted to highlight that. And then now, thank you for indulging me.


In Vitro Fertilization And Zygote Epigenetics

Transmission of Sexual Choice (02:04:48)

And let's, if you will, let's continue down this path that we were going on. Because I want to make sure that we absolutely get to this issue of transmission of information about sex choice of offspring. So the worms are her muscle diets, which means that they make both sperm and eggs. But they are also males, which are much more rare. And they can choose to mate with their males or not. And when they mate with the male, it's a huge decision. Because it's very costly, energetically. And they also risk predation and all kinds of troubles. The males hurt them and reduce their lives when they mate with them. People are going to draw all sorts of analogies here. It's inevitable, but hey, here we go. And most importantly, for evolution, when you mate with another animal, you dilute your genome in half. Because the worms can just self-fertilize and transmit the exact same genome to the next generation. But when they mate, they dilute it in half. So this is a big price to pay. On the other hand, when you mate, you diversify your genome. So maybe some combination of gene will be good. And we know that in humans, I mean, it's kind of interesting that the brain circuits that are associated with a version and with approach are fairly hardwired for a number of things. Like puddle of vomit, almost everybody kind of hinges, plate of cookies. If you like cookies, you move towards it. But there's one particular word in the English and Brazilian Israeli language that ought to evoke disgust. And that's incest. Because incest is actually not just disgusting as a practice, but it's dangerous genetically. Because inbreeding creates a deleterious mutation. So there are studies on how people in Israeli kibbutz, for example, where they all grow together. The children live together. They just do be like that. Don't date each other because this is the classic thing. I talked to some of whom the kibbutz told me that's not true, but yes, there are studies like this that say, but it makes sense. And in some countries, Scandinavians are in a lapland and isolem where populations are small. They keep exquisite records of lineage in order to avoid inbreeding. Right. So you're absolutely right. But the worms, it's the safe choice for them is to self-mate. And if they mate with a male, they take a risk, but they diverse it. Okay. What we found is that if you take the hermaphrodite, we can call it the female for just one second. And you stress it with high temperatures. Then the next generations of worms for free generations mate much more with males. And they do it because the female starts secreting a pheromone that attracts the males. Ah, it's very cryptic mechanism. It's not that she somehow changes and then goes seeking males. No. It draws males. It draws men. We know how it works. We think we know how it works. What happens is that the stress, the high temperatures, compromise the production of sperm in the hermaphrodites. So the hermaphrodite, they make sperm enough to make generations. But the sperm, because of defective, smaller in the heritats, because the RNA is not inherited, okay? The sperm is not made optimally. So they make less sperm. And when they don't make a lot of sperm, they feel that they don't self-fertilize correctly. So they call the males by secreting the pheromones so that it would provide its own sperm and they can continue to make babies. Okay. And we know this also from the experiment, you just take a hermaphrodite and you kill its sperm, start secreting a pheromone and the males come. It's a need-based system. Exactly. Incredible. And I hope people can appreciate as they're hearing this that none of this, we assume, I don't know how to speak worm, none of this we assume is a conscious decision in these animals. Much like human mating behavior, which to us always seem so conscious, but is being governed by both conscious and subconscious decision-making. None of this is an active decision to secrete the hormone to draw in more males. It's simply a biasing of probabilities. The hormone is now secreted in greater quantities or greater frequency. The males therefore approach more. So it's just increasing probability of interactions. Is that right? Right. What happens naturally, normally, if you don't stress the ancestors, is that the worms start secreting the pheromone only when they are old. These also know people will- When they're running out of their own fertility. Exactly. Because they only make the sperm at a particular time and then they run out of the stem, they can self-servelize. So they have to call the males if they want to continue to mate. Well, that's just sort of the plastic surgery approach. Okay, I'll take the heat for that one. But it's true. I think as certain people age to a certain point and they feel that their fertility is waning, if they want offspring, they need to take any number of different approaches. They could get a- here we're talking about a female, but we could also do the reverse. Right? We do sperm donor, right? But if they want to attract a lifelong mate or co-parent with somebody, often times they will do things to adjust their attractiveness in any number of different ways. Psychological attractiveness or physical attractiveness. I'm not afraid to bring this up because I think that the parallels are very important because I do think that every species and individuals within a species, of course, decides whether or not they want to reproduce or not, but has an inherent understanding, conscious or subconscious, about where they reside in the arc of their lifespan. I do believe that, not just based on experience. Some people are very attuned to the passage of time being very fast, others very slow. I think that knowing how long your parents and their parents lived makes a big difference. I have friends whose fathers in particular died fairly young and all these guys basically got married and had kids really young. Right. So here, luckily for me, I don't have to get into psychology of the worms. The explanation is just like an instinct when they run out of sperm, they start secating the ferromons and attract the males. There are studies also in new men's about older fathers. The children of older fathers have more- has a higher chance of becoming autistic. They are studying? 40 and up, basically. However, in this case, it's not clear that this is something epigenetics could be just because of DNA damage, because it accumulates. And actually nowadays, we have an episode on fertility coming up, both male and female fertility, and there are actually DNA fragmentation kits. At home, DNA fragmentation kits are sperm analysis. You send the sperm back in, you don't do the DNA. People pipetting, semen at home, wouldn't be an odd picture. Let's not go there. But there are clinics that do this for a nominal charge.


Fertility & Human Disease; 3-Parent In Vitro Fertilization (IVF); RNA Testing (02:11:22)

But I did want to ask about autism and human disease in particular. Another thing that you hear sometimes, and here I want to acknowledge autism is on a spectrum. Some people get upset if you call it a disorder. There are some adaptive autistic traits and et cetera. But one thing that often comes up is this idea that two people who are more of the kind of engineering, hard science, if you will, of phenotype, mate and have children, higher probability of the offspring being on the spectrum. Some people would argue, ah, but that's already selecting for people that might have already been partially on the spectrum. So maybe it's a gene copy issue. I'm not asking you to comment on autism in particular. But when you hear things like that, the children of older fathers, more and from older fathers, tend higher probability of autism, what is that at the level of intuition? Does that strike you as an epigenetic phenomenon, as a nature nurture mishmash, or the possibility that it's RNA passage, or anything? Does anything sort of trigger the whiskers, your spidey sense? So in that case, I would go with the most personal stimulus explanation, which is just less fidelity or less DNA maintenance and some damage that passes on. It doesn't have to be an epigenetic thing. But the sperm are generated at once every 60 days. So the damage must be at the level of the germ cells not having the proper machinery, mitochondria or something like that. Or the DNA repair machinery. The DNA repair machinery could be defective, or could work less well in older people, leading to the constant production of germ cells with more mutation. This is a possibility. Do we know exactly what the DNA repair machinery is? Yes. There are many types of DNA repair. There's one that uses other copies of the DNA to correct. There are ones that just recognize all kinds of lesions on the DNA and remove it. It's very elaborate and complicated system. And is it a system that is now tractable, that can be modified through pharmacology or through anything like that? So I don't know about drugs that correct, that improve it. Maybe they exist and I'm not aware that it's very well understood. And many people are studying these directions. Yeah. One thing that came across in the exploration of the fertility work is that what I'm about to describe is not legal in the U.S. It is illegal, but is illegal in the U.K. and in other countries is this notion of three parent IVF, where it does seem that some of the eggs that persist in older females even if fertilized don't produce healthy embryos, they have chromosomal abnormalities or replications and deletions that are problematic for the development of the embryo, such as trisomy 21, aka Down syndrome, in part or in large part because of deficits in the mitochondrial genome. So what they now do is they take the, because the mitochondrial genome resides mainly in the cytoplasm, they'll take the, an egg from the mother, the sperm from the father, but they'll take the nucleus from the mother and put that into a cytoplasm of a younger woman whose mitochondrial DNA is healthy, then use the sperm to fertilize that egg, and that's why it's called three parent IVF. Then implant that into the mother, and this has been done several times for, in cases of mitochondrial damage or mutations in the mother. It works, the question is whether or not those offspring will grow up to be healthy. So this, of course, is not just a pure divergence, it raises a bigger question that I have for you, which is in terms of the work in either C. elegans or in other model organisms, but in particular in C. elegans, where do you see this going next? And if you would indulge us, I would love you to tell us a little bit about the admittedly unpublished work that you're doing on temperature, exposure, and environments. I mean, how malleable is this system? Because to me, it just seems incredibly malleable, and yet a lot of it is still cloaked off to us, there's still a ton to learn. So, assuming that we will discover similar things in humans, which we don't know that this is the case, but let's say we find it. I think there are many things you can do before you change it. For example, you could also change a variant inheritance by having the parent exercise, for example. Some things like this have been done, for example, and there are experiments in rodents where they show that overfeeding the rodents creates problems for the next generation, for the children. However, if you let the rodent exercise, then it corrects the aberrant inheritance. So, this is one possibility, and you can also manipulate it at the source. You can change, if it's RNAs, let's say you could in the future, perhaps, if we understand how it works, actually change the composition of the heritaval RNAs. By eating RNAs, just like the worms, or RNA sandwich? No, so the RNA sandwich will be difficult because it's not, I don't know, but if you do IVS, if you do any bit of fertilization, you can perhaps change the composition of the RNAs in the stuff that you introduce. But way before that, what you could do, perhaps, even in the not so far future, is use this for diagnostics. DNA-based diagnostics for every couple that wants to have a kid. In Israel, this is done for most couples. You can look at the DNA and look for genetic disease. But no one is looking at the RNA at the moment. If you understand how it works better, we'll have another level, a whole new world, to look at. And perhaps there will be some RNAs that correlate with disease that will say, okay, you know, the beauty is that this unlike DNA, it's plastic. So with DNA, this is your DNA, perhaps we can choose another embryo. But here you could say, perhaps, or again, the future, this is science fiction, doesn't happen now, but if we understand this and it's true, we can say, maybe you should run on the treadmill a little bit, this will change the profile of your RNAs, and then we will use it for IVF. This seems more because just it correlates with healthy profiles of RNAs. This is a level that no one looks at now and holds great potential. Again, with a disclaimer that we know how it works in humans at all. Yeah. But of course, it's so interesting.


Deliberate Cold Exposure, Learning & Memory (02:17:56)

Yeah, it's super interesting, incredibly promising. So along the lines of things that one can do in the short term, and your experiments on C. elegans, I'd love for you to share with us what you're observing about cold exposure and how that impacts subsequent generations of C. elegans. And if you would indulge us with the story of this discovery, like some of the earlier stories you told us, it is a surprising and fascinating one. I'll gladly tell you about this. This is not a story about transgenerational inheritance. It's a story about memory within one generation. Ah, excuse me. Within one generation, okay? And as you said, the story about how it happens is it's totally by accident. It's a funny story. And I'm bringing this up because I know Dana Lanchoff, who's a huge fan of your post office, really, you know, be happy. This is her work. This is her work, and this is unpublished work. We didn't even finish it, so we're working on it. Okay, well, when it's published, we will feature the paper because I love this story. Oh, thanks a lot. Great. So what happened is that when you -- we talked about transgenerational memories, and I said that in worms, there are very long transgenerational memories. If a generation time for serialgance is three days, some memory is less for many generations. So way beyond the lifespan of the worm. The lifespan of the worm is three weeks, okay? You have a new generation that will be three days, but every worm lives for three weeks. But there's a lot of research that shows that unlike heritable memory, which can be very long, the memory that the worms acquired during the lifetime is very short-lived. So if you teach something, after two hours, it forgets. Okay? So, for example, you can teach the worm. You can take an auto that it likes and pair it with starvation, and then it would dislike the auto. And then there's a simple test. You just put it in a plate. You put the auto in one side and a control auto in the other side, and you see whether it prefers this auto or not, and it stops preferring it. Okay? There's 30 years or more of research, 40 years of research on this, showing that the worms forget after two hours. The reason I went to study serialgance is that I wanted to understand memory, because such a simple nervous system, you say, "Maybe I have the potential to actually understand how it works." But this is slightly disappointing because they forget after two hours. So what is it exactly? Okay? My idea was, and I tried to convince students to do it for ten years, is to take the worms, teach them this association to dislike the auto that they innately like, and then just put the worms in -80 and freeze them completely, throw them and see whether they still remember after their thought. The Han Solo experiment. And I didn't want to do it because of cryo-preservation or something like this. I wanted to do it because, as you know better than me, many theories about memory say that you need electrical activity to maintain the memory. So you need to reverberate it in the brain. During dreams or replay of the thing or whatever. And if the memories will never be less be kept, even though the worms were frozen in -80, it would mean that it was kept in the absence of electricity, because there's no electricity in -80 degrees. This was the idea. I asked many students, no one wanted to do it, because it's not so easy and also a little crazy. Well, and when the PI, the principal investigator lab has a pet experiment, no one wanted to do that experiment. That is the university tool. So, and then I agreed to do it, done a lunch. I was very happy, only later to find out that she ignored me completely and did a different experiment. The experiment the data did instead is to just take the worms, teach them the association and place them on ice. She wanted to see how the kinetics of memory and forgetting change in low temperature, because maybe whatever memory is, the breakdown of the memory is affected by the temperature. A very simple idea. Different experiment. Different experiment. Yeah. By a cool experiment. Very cool. And what she found is that when you place the worms on ice, after you teach them, they just don't forget. If they're even 10 times longer than controlled worms. At that point, after 24 hours, if no one worms forget after 2 hours, after 24 hours, the worms will become 6. So normally we do shorter experiments. Okay. But for 2 hours, the worms don't forget. This is cool, but it was only the beginning. Because the boring explanation is just what I just said. Everything slows down in low temperatures. So the breakdown of memory, again, we don't know what it is, but whatever it is, happens slower in low temperatures. But this is not the case. It's not merely the physical. It's the response. It's the changing of the internal state of the worms, which affects the memory kinetics. How do we know this? There's a beautiful work of the last one years on cold tolerance in theoregance nematodes. If you take the worms and you place them on ice, like she did, but longer, for 48 hours, they all die. However, if you take the worms, acclimate them to lower temperatures for a few hours, 5 hours in the minimum, and then place them on ice, they all survive. They become cold tolerance, and people who study this show that this involves changes in lipid metabolism and many things. So Dana took the worms, acclimated them to slightly lower temperatures, made them cold, resistant, and then taught them the association and placed them on ice. And now they forgot immediately, which means that when they change their internal state to become cold tolerance, they no longer extend memories on ice, which means it's not only the temperature, because the temperature was at any way low. Now they know the moment. We took this as a starting point to understand which genes change when the worms are becoming cold tolerance on and off ice. And we found genes that when you mutate them, the worms just remember the longer always, even when they're off ice, because these are the genes that normally change when they're surprised on the ice. And these genes are expressed just in one pair of noirons, just two out of the 302. Now as he said, 302, not 300. And we can manipulate the activities of these genes in these noirons to extend memory. And then the punchline of everything that happened is that we found out that this noiron where this genes function, this one pair of noirons, is the only known in celicants, which is sensitive to lithium. And lithium is a drug that is being given to bipolar with other patients for decades. Although it's not entirely clear how it works. It's very, very interesting. It's also interesting. There's an episode, of course, in your podcast about this, you know more about this than me a lot. But it's also interesting because it's just an atom created in the big bank, yet it works on our brains in such a fundamental way. And we wanted to see whether it works on the world, because this noiron was tied to this memory extension phenotype that we found. So Dan and Gerudo worms only tuned, removed them from lithium, caught them the association and found out that they learn a lot longer than control worms. Not only that, if you first make the worms call tolerant, then lithium doesn't work on them. So lithium switches this forgetfulness mechanism on and off. Amazing. And it's all connected to call tolerance. Amazing. And amazing for a number of reasons. And so at risk of being long-winded in my response, I just wanted to highlight something that I think will be of relevance to most people, which is when at some point we did a few episodes on memory. And I highlighted a review that was written by the great James McGaugh, one of the great mammalian memory researchers, who's worked a lot on humans and mice. And I was shocked, pun intended, and amused to learn that in medieval times, if people wanted children to remember lessons, they could be religious lessons or school doctrine or whatever it was, mathematics, whether they would take children, teach them, and then throw them into cold water to introduce a memory instilling event. And we now know that the memory instilling event is the release of adrenaline in the body, which makes perfect sense if you think about traumatic events. But it also, this whole general mechanism also applies to the learning of other types of information. And so if I understand correctly about the role of lithium and the role of cold in the experiments that you just described, there's some general state switch, some internal state switch that says what happened in the minutes or hours preceding this was important. It acts as sort of like a highlighter pen in the book of experiences. And I'm absolutely curious to know whether or not this is an RNA-dependent mechanism in some way. So is this literally like the highlighter in the IKEA instruction book? This we don't know. This we don't know. And as I said, this is not even a finished work. It's not peer reviewed. It's just the state that I told you about. But it's very exciting for me to go into this new field. And once it's out, I'd be happy to talk more about it and think about the implications and the connections to other things and more about the mechanism. Yeah. Well, thank you for sharing with us despite the fact that it's not finished. People now know that it's also not finished. And I love a good cliffhanger. So we await the full conclusion and interpretation of these results. Today, you've taken us on an amazing journey through the genome RNA, short interfering RNAs, a ton of history of prior experiments, some of which ended tragically, many of which unfortunately did not. And they were true triumphs in particular the work in your laboratory, which is just incredible. And also this introduction of model organisms. So, and I only mentioned a short handful of the things that you've taught us about today. So first I want to extend thanks for the incredible teaching. I also want to say thank you for something equally important, which is that absolutely came through, but is what initially brought me to explore you and your work more, although I had certainly heard of you, which is that your spirit and kind of approach to biology is an extremely unique and intoxicating one. It's even, I venture to call it seductive. You know, there's a, I do believe that whether or not it's music or poetry or science or mathematics, that the spirit behind something dictates the amount of intelligence and precision with which that thing is carried out. And it absolutely comes through. So if I'm making a feel on the spot about this, I've succeeded. Thank you very much. But I know that the listeners can feel it. It's a felt thing. So thank you. There are many scientists out there, fewer with this phenotype and even fewer that, you know, I think that can communicate with such articulate precision. So thank you so much. Thank you. It's been a real pleasure. Pleasure was all mine. Thanks for the long. Great. Well, we'll do it again and we'll learn about all the incredible things you're doing, trying to transform science as it were at the level of publishing at the level of social media, because there's a whole other discussion there. Meanwhile, we will, of course, point people in the direction of you and to learn more about your work. And I look forward to hearing the conclusion of Dana's studies. Thanks a lot.


Public Engagement And Support

Zero-Cost Support, Spotify & Apple Reviews, YouTube Feedback, Sponsors, Momentous, Social Media, Neural Network Newsletter (02:29:26)

It's been a real pleasure. Thank you for joining me today for my discussion with Dr. Oded Rakhavi about genetics, inheritance, the epigenome, and transgenerational passage of traits. If you're learning from and are enjoying the podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube channel. That's a terrific zero cost way to support us. In addition, please subscribe to the podcast on Spotify and Apple. And on both Spotify and Apple, you can leave us up to a five-star review. If you have questions for us or comments or suggestions about topics you'd like us to cover or guess you'd like me to include on the Huberman Lab podcast, please put those in the comment section on YouTube. I do read all the comments. In addition, please check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning and throughout today's episode. That's the best way to support this podcast. Not so much on today's episode, but on many previous episodes of the Huberman Lab podcast, we discuss supplements. While supplements aren't necessary for everybody, many people derive tremendous benefit from them for things like improving sleep, improving focus, and for hormone support. The Huberman Lab podcast is proud to partner with momentous supplements. We've done that for several reasons. First of all, momentous supplements are the very highest quality. Also, they tend to focus on single ingredient formulations, which makes it very straightforward to build the most cost effective and biologically effective supplement regimen for you. We've also partnered with momentous because we realize that many listeners of this podcast reside outside of the United States and fortunately momentous ships internationally. If you'd like to learn more about the supplements discussed on the Huberman Lab podcast, you can go to Live Momentous spelled O-U-S so that's livemomentous.com/huberman. If you're not already following us on social media, please feel free to do so. We are Huberman Lab on Instagram, Twitter, Facebook, and LinkedIn. And at all those places, I cover information, some of which overlaps with the content of the Huberman Lab podcast, but much of which is distinct from content on the Huberman Lab podcast. So again, it's Huberman Lab on all social media channels. The Huberman Lab podcast also has a so-called neural network newsletter. This is a zero cost monthly newsletter that consists of podcast summaries and so-called toolkits. And those toolkits range from things like a toolkit for sleep, for focus, for controlling dopamine, deliberate exposure, for increasing focus, and much, much more. If you want to sign up for that newsletter, you can go to HubermanLab.com, go to the menu, scroll down to newsletter, you simply provide your email, and I assure you, we do not share your email with anybody. And again, it's a completely zero cost newsletter, find it at HubermanLab.com.


Great! You’ve successfully signed up.

Welcome back! You've successfully signed in.

You've successfully subscribed to Wisdom In a Nutshell.

Success! Check your email for magic link to sign-in.

Success! Your billing info has been updated.

Your billing was not updated.